Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Pragmatically Framed Cross-Situational Noun Learning Using Computational Reinforcement Models.Shamima Najnin & Bonny Banerjee - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Integrating constraints for learning word–referent mappings.Padraic Monaghan & Karen Mattock - 2012 - Cognition 123 (1):133-143.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Gavagai Is as Gavagai Does: Learning Nouns and Verbs From Cross‐Situational Statistics.Padraic Monaghan, Karen Mattock, Robert A. I. Davies & Alastair C. Smith - 2015 - Cognitive Science 39 (5):1099-1112.
    Learning to map words onto their referents is difficult, because there are multiple possibilities for forming these mappings. Cross-situational learning studies have shown that word-object mappings can be learned across multiple situations, as can verbs when presented in a syntactic context. However, these previous studies have presented either nouns or verbs in ambiguous contexts and thus bypass much of the complexity of multiple grammatical categories in speech. We show that noun word learning in adults is robust when objects are moving, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The Role of Novelty in Early Word Learning.Emily Mather & Kim Plunkett - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (7):1157-1177.
    What mechanism implements the mutual exclusivity bias to map novel labels to objects without names? Prominent theoretical accounts of mutual exclusivity (e.g., Markman, 1989, 1990) propose that infants are guided by their knowledge of object names. However, the mutual exclusivity constraint could be implemented via monitoring of object novelty (see Merriman, Marazita, & Jarvis, 1995). We sought to discriminate between these contrasting explanations across two preferential looking experiments with 22-month-olds. In Experiment 1, infants viewed three objects: one name-known, two name-unknown. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Object‐Label‐Order Effect When Learning From an Inconsistent Source.Timmy Ma & Natalia L. Komarova - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (8):e12737.
    Learning in natural environments is often characterized by a degree of inconsistency from an input. These inconsistencies occur, for example, when learning from more than one source, or when the presence of environmental noise distorts incoming information; as a result, the task faced by the learner becomes ambiguous. In this study, we investigate how learners handle such situations. We focus on the setting where a learner receives and processes a sequence of utterances to master associations between objects and their labels, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Dynamic Self-organizing and early lexical Development in children.Ping Li, Xiaowei Zhao & Brian Mac Whinney - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (4):581-612.
    In this study we present a self‐organizing connectionist model of early lexical development. We call this model DevLex‐II, based on the earlier DevLex model. DevLex‐II can simulate a variety of empirical patterns in children's acquisition of words. These include a clear vocabulary spurt, effects of word frequency and length on age of acquisition, and individual differences as a function of phonological short‐term memory and associative capacity. Further results from lesioned models indicate developmental plasticity in the network's recovery from damage, in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Lexical Organization and Competition in First and Second Languages: Computational and Neural Mechanisms.Ping Li - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (4):629-664.
    How does a child rapidly acquire and develop a structured mental organization for the vast number of words in the first years of life? How does a bilingual individual deal with the even more complicated task of learning and organizing two lexicons? It is only until recently have we started to examine the lexicon as a dynamical system with regard to its acquisition, representation, and organization. In this article, I outline a proposal based on our research that takes the dynamical (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Dynamic Self‐Organization and Early Lexical Development in Children.Ping Li, Xiaowei Zhao & Brian Mac Whinney - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (4):581-612.
    In this study we present a self-organizing connectionist model of early lexical development. We call this model DevLex-II, based on the earlier DevLex model. DevLex-II can simulate a variety of empirical patterns in children's acquisition of words. These include a clear vocabulary spurt, effects of word frequency and length on age of acquisition, and individual differences as a function of phonological short-term memory and associative capacity. Further results from lesioned models indicate developmental plasticity in the network's recovery from damage, in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • The Interplay of Cross‐Situational Word Learning and Sentence‐Level Constraints.Judith Koehne & Matthew W. Crocker - 2015 - Cognitive Science 39 (5):849-889.
    A variety of mechanisms contribute to word learning. Learners can track co-occurring words and referents across situations in a bottom-up manner. Equally, they can exploit sentential contexts, relying on top–down information such as verb–argument relations and world knowledge, offering immediate constraints on meaning. When combined, CSWL and SLCL potentially modulate each other's influence, revealing how word learners deal with multiple mechanisms simultaneously: Do they use all mechanisms? Prefer one? Is their strategy context dependent? Three experiments conducted with adult learners reveal (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • A Bootstrapping Model of Frequency and Context Effects in Word Learning.Kachergis George, Yu Chen & M. Shiffrin Richard - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (3):590-622.
    Prior research has shown that people can learn many nouns from a short series of ambiguous situations containing multiple words and objects. For successful cross-situational learning, people must approximately track which words and referents co-occur most frequently. This study investigates the effects of allowing some word-referent pairs to appear more frequently than others, as is true in real-world learning environments. Surprisingly, high-frequency pairs are not always learned better, but can also boost learning of other pairs. Using a recent associative model, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Goldilocks Forgetting in Cross-Situational Learning.Paul Ibbotson, Diana G. López & Alan J. McKane - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:387015.
    Given that there is referential uncertainty (noise) when learning words, to what extent can forgetting filter some of that noise out, and be an aid to learning? Using a Cross Situational Learning model we find a U-shaped function of errors indicative of a “Goldilocks” zone of forgetting: an optimum store-loss ratio that is neither too aggressive or too weak, but just the right amount to produce better learning outcomes. Forgetting acts as a high-pass filter that actively deletes (part of) the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cross-situational learning in a Zipfian environment.Andrew T. Hendrickson & Amy Perfors - 2019 - Cognition 189 (C):11-22.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Easy Words: Reference Resolution in a Malevolent Referent World.Lila R. Gleitman & John C. Trueswell - 2018 - Topics in Cognitive Science 12 (1):22-47.
    Gleitman and Trueswell’s “The easy words” forms a pair with their earlier paper, “Hard words,” completing a circle in which the authors ask how “easy” words (e.g., concrete nouns) are learned. They take up the hypothesis of “cross‐situational learning,” and argue that accumulating observations actually hinders learning if the mechanism requires holding all exemplars in memory over time. They present an alternative hypothesis, “Propose but Verify,” wherein people use one‐trial learning to confirm or disconfirm their current hypothesis—a mechanism distinctly different (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • A Computational Model for the Item‐Based Induction of Construction Networks.Judith Gaspers & Philipp Cimiano - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (3):439-488.
    According to usage‐based approaches to language acquisition, linguistic knowledge is represented in the form of constructions—form‐meaning pairings—at multiple levels of abstraction and complexity. The emergence of syntactic knowledge is assumed to be a result of the gradual abstraction of lexically specific and item‐based linguistic knowledge. In this article, we explore how the gradual emergence of a network consisting of constructions at varying degrees of complexity can be modeled computationally. Linguistic knowledge is learned by observing natural language utterances in an ambiguous (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Prediction error boosts retention of novel words in adults but not in children.Chiara Gambi, Martin J. Pickering & Hugh Rabagliati - 2021 - Cognition 211 (C):104650.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Grammar induction by unification of type-logical lexicons.Sean A. Fulop - 2010 - Journal of Logic, Language and Information 19 (3):353-381.
    A method is described for inducing a type-logical grammar from a sample of bare sentence trees which are annotated by lambda terms, called term-labelled trees . Any type logic from a permitted class of multimodal logics may be specified for use with the procedure, which induces the lexicon of the grammar including the grammatical categories. A first stage of semantic bootstrapping is performed, which induces a general form lexicon from the sample of term-labelled trees using Fulop’s (J Log Lang Inf (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning to talk about events from narrated video in a construction grammar framework.Dominey Peter Ford & Jean-David Boucher - 2005 - Artificial Intelligence 167 (1-2):31-61.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Cross-situational and supervised learning in the emergence of communication.Jose Fernando Fontanari & Angelo Cangelosi - 2011 - Interaction Studies 12 (1):119-133.
    Scenarios for the emergence or bootstrap of a lexicon involve the repeated interaction between at least two agents who must reach a consensus on how to name N objects using H words. Here we consider minimal models of two types of learning algorithms: cross-situational learning, in which the individuals determine the meaning of a word by looking for something in common across all observed uses of that word, and supervised operant conditioning learning, in which there is strong feedback between individuals (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Developmental Changes in Cross‐Situational Word Learning: The Inverse Effect of Initial Accuracy.Stanka A. Fitneva & Morten H. Christiansen - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (S1):141-161.
    Intuitively, the accuracy of initial word-referent mappings should be positively correlated with the outcome of learning. Yet recent evidence suggests an inverse effect of initial accuracy in adults, whereby greater accuracy of initial mappings is associated with poorer outcomes in a cross-situational learning task. Here, we examine the impact of initial accuracy on 4-year-olds, 10-year-olds, and adults. For half of the participants most word-referent mappings were initially correct and for the other half most mappings were initially incorrect. Initial accuracy was (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A computational theory of child overextension.Renato Ferreira Pinto & Yang Xu - 2021 - Cognition 206:104472.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive science in the era of artificial intelligence: A roadmap for reverse-engineering the infant language-learner.Emmanuel Dupoux - 2018 - Cognition 173 (C):43-59.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Names, Descriptions, and Assertion.Ray Buchanan - 2014 - In Zsu-Wei Hung (ed.), Communicative Action. Springer. pp. 03-15.
    According to Millian Descriptivism, while the semantic content of a linguistically simple proper name is just its referent, we often use sentences containing such expressions “to make assertions…that are, in part, descriptive” (Soames 2008). Against this view, I show, following Ted Sider and David Braun (2006), that simple sentences containing names are never used to assert descriptively enriched propositions. In addition, I offer a diagnosis as to where the argument for Millian Descriptivism goes wrong. Once we appreciate the distinctive way (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Language Evolution and Robotics: Issues on Symbol Grounding.Paul Vogt - 2006 - In A. Loula, R. Gudwin & J. Queiroz (eds.), Artificial Cognition Systems. Idea Group Publishers. pp. 176.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark