- There is no “point” to space.Gary W. Strong - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):279-279.details
|
|
What does calibration solve?Arnold Trehub - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):279-280.details
|
|
The “calibration” solution still leaves much work to be done.A. P. Petrov - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):273-274.details
|
|
Is λ an appropriate control variable for locomotion?Thomas M. Hamm & Zong-Sheng Han - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):761-762.details
|
|
The unobservability of central commands: Why testing hypotheses is so difficult.Antony Hodgson - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):763-764.details
|
|
Grip force adjustments during rapid hand movements suggest that detailed movement kinematics are predicted.J. Randall Flanagan, James R. Tresilian & Alan M. Wing - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):753-754.details
|
|
Reciprocal and coactivation commands are not sufficient to describe muscle activation patterns.C. C. A. M. Gielen & B. van Bolhuis - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):754-755.details
|
|
Why neural synchrony fails to explain the unity of visual consciousness.Eric LaRock - 2006 - Behavior and Philosophy 34:39-58.details
|
|
Objective Subjectivity: Allocentric and Egocentric Representations in Thought and Experience.Pete Mandik - 2000 - Dissertation, Washington Universitydetails
|
|
Mental representation and the subjectivity of consciousness.Pete Mandik - 2001 - Philosophical Psychology 14 (2):179-202.details
|
|
How Subjects Can Emerge from Neurons.Eric LaRock & Mostyn Jones - 2019 - Process Studies 48 (1):40-58.details
|
|
A mechanism for spatial perception on human skin.Francesca Fardo, Brianna Beck, Tony Cheng & Patrick Haggard - 2018 - Cognition 178 (C):236-243.details
|
|
Why Vision is More than Seeing.Melvyn A. Goodale - 2001 - Canadian Journal of Philosophy 31 (sup1):186-214.details
|
|
Vector code in space constancy.E. N. Sokolov - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):278-278.details
|
|
The translation solution plus motion suppression account for perceived stability.Arnold E. Stoper - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):278-279.details
|
|
Neuronal death of the cancellation theory?Claude Prablanc - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):274-275.details
|
|
Just how different are perceptual and visuomotor localization abilities?Paul Dassonville, John Schlag & Madeleine Schlag-Rey - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):258-259.details
|
|
How far should we extend the equilibrium point (lambda) hypothesis?Jack M. Winters - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):785-786.details
|
|
Interneurons as backseat drivers and the elusive control variable.T. Richard Nichols - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):772-773.details
|
|
Can the λ model benefit from understanding human adaptation in weightlessness(and vice versa)?P. Vernon McDonald - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):768-768.details
|
|
Do control variables exist?Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos & William H. Warren - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):762-762.details
|
|
Shifting frames of reference but the same old point of view.Gerald L. Gottlieb - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):758-758.details
|
|
Streams and consciousness: Visual awareness and the brain.A. David Milner - 1998 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2 (1):25-30.details
|
|
On the locus of visual stability.Daniel N. Robinson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):275-276.details
|
|
Equifinality and phase-resetting: The role of control parameter manipulations.R. E. A. van Emmerik & R. C. Wagenaar - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):783-784.details
|
|
The lambda model is only one piece in the motor control puzzle.Jeffrey Dean - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):749-749.details
|
|
Is the multi-joint pointing movement model applicable to equilibrium control during upper trunk movements?Alexey Alexandrov, Alexander Frolov & Jean Massion - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-746.details
|
|
The origin and use of positional frames of reference in motor control.Anatol G. Feldman & Mindy F. Levin - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):723-744.details
|
|
Body-centered representations for visually-guided action emerge during early infancy.Rick O. Gilmore & Mark H. Johnson - 1997 - Cognition 65 (1):B1-B9.details
|
|
How our world remains stable despite disturbing influences.Bruce Bridgeman, A. H. C. Van der Heijden & Boris M. Velichkovsky - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):282-292.details
|
|
The world as an outside iconic memory – no strong internal metric means no problem of visual stability.J. Kevin O'Regan - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):270-271.details
|
|
Is there a role for extraretinal factors in the maintenance of stability in a structured environment?Eugene Chekaluk - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):258-258.details
|
|
Levers to generate movement.U. Windhorst - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):784-785.details
|
|
Kinematic invariances and body schema.Pietro Morasso & Vittorio Sanguineti - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):769-770.details
|
|
Seeing where we look: Fixation as extraretinal information.D. Alfred Owens & Edward S. Reed - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):271-272.details
|
|
A theory of visual stability across saccadic eye movements.Bruce Bridgeman, A. H. C. Van der Heijden & Boris M. Velichkovsky - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):247-258.details
|
|
Equilibrium-point control? Yes! Deterministic mechanisms of control? No!Mark L. Latash - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):765-766.details
|
|
A few reasons why psychologlsts can adhere to Feldman and Levin's model.Mireille Bonnard & Jean Pailhous - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):746-747.details
|
|
Studying Spatial Conceptualization across Cultures: Anthropology and Cognitive Science.Stephen C. Levinson - 1998 - Ethos: Journal of the Society for Psychological Anthropology 26 (1):7-24.details
|
|
Frameworks on shifting sands.R. Lngvaldsen & H. T. A. Whiting - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):764-765.details
|
|
Perceptual stability and postsaccadic visual information: Can man bridge a gap?H. Deubel & W. X. Schneider - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):259-260.details
|
|
What can we expect from models of motor control?Gerald E. Loeb - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):767-768.details
|
|
(1 other version)The central role of the parietal lobes in consciousness.John G. Taylor - 2001 - Consciousness and Cognition 10 (3):379-417.details
|
|
Fixations or smooth eye movements?Alexander H. Wertheim - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):281-282.details
|
|
Task dependent spatial memory across saccades.Keith S. Karn, Joel Lachter, Per Møller & Mary Hayhoe - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):267-268.details
|
|
Control parameters, equilibria, and coordination dynamics.Dagmar Sternad & M. T. Turvey - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):780-780.details
|
|
Spatial frames for motor control would be commensurate with spatial frames for vision and proprioception, but what about control of energy flows?Christopher C. Pagano & Geoffrey P. Bingham - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):773-773.details
|
|
Moving models of motion forward: Explication and a new concept.Thomas G. Fikes & James T. Townsend - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):751-753.details
|
|
Visual stability: What is new?P. van Donkelaar & U. Windhorst - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):280-281.details
|
|
The λ model for motor control: More than meets the eye.Mindy F. Levin & Anatol G. Feldman - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):786-806.details
|
|