- Liberal Representationalism: A Deflationist Defense.Marc Artiga - 2016 - Dialectica 70 (3):407-430.details
|
|
The ontogenesis of narrative: from moving to meaning.Jonathan T. Delafield-Butt & Colwyn Trevarthen - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.details
|
|
A teleofunctional account of evolutionary mismatch.Nathan Cofnas - 2016 - Biology and Philosophy 31 (4):507-525.details
|
|
Gricean Communication, Joint Action, and the Evolution of Cooperation.Richard Moore - 2018 - Topoi 37 (2):329-341.details
|
|
Darwin’s Rehabilitation of Teleology Versus Williams’ Replacement of Teleology by Natural Selection.Harry Smit - 2010 - Biological Theory 5 (4):357-365.details
|
|
Can a sociobiology of mind discard the will?Ian Vine - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):318-319.details
|
|
Toward an empirical foundation for evolutionary psychology.David M. Buss - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):301-302.details
|
|
Genetically determined neural modules versus mental constructional acts in the genesis of human intelligence.Kathleen R. Gibson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):308-309.details
|
|
Calls as labels: An intriguing theme, but one with limitations.Donald H. Owings - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):162-163.details
|
|
Knowing thyself, knowing the other: They're not the same.Jonathan Schull & J. David Smith - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):166-167.details
|
|
Monkeys and consciousness.D. M. Armstrong - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):147-148.details
|
|
Cerebellar contributions to response selection.Gary G. Berntson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):214-215.details
|
|
The mobility gradient: Useful, general, falsifiable?John A. Byers - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):270-271.details
|
|
Eshkol-Wachman movement notation and the evolution of locomotor patterns in vertebrates.Robert C. Eaton - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):272-274.details
|
|
Motives: Metaphors in motion.John C. Fentress - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):219-219.details
|
|
The natural geometry of a behavioral homology.Ilan Golani - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):291-308.details
|
|
What are voluntary movements made of?Ian Q. Whishaw - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):290-291.details
|
|
Tentative analysis of apomorphine-induced intraspecific aggressive behavior in the rat according to Adams's classification.B. Senault - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):226-227.details
|
|
Learning theory in its niche.Howard Rachlin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):155-156.details
|
|
Adaptive modification of behavior: Processing information from the environment.Wolfgang M. Schleidt - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):158-159.details
|
|
An ecological approach toward a unified theory of learning.William R. Charlesworth - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):142-143.details
|
|
The relevance of phylogenetics to the study of behavioral diversity.Michael T. Ghiselin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):144-145.details
|
|
The ecological approach to learning.John Kruse & Edward Reed - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):148-149.details
|
|
Learning theory: Behavioral artifacts or general principles?John A. Nevin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):152-153.details
|
|
EEG desynchronization is associated with cellular events that are prerequisites for active behavioral states.M. Steriade - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):489-492.details
|
|
A theory in need of defense.Marc N. Branch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):678-679.details
|
|
Avoidance is in the head, not the genes.Everett J. Wyers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):685-685.details
|
|
The defense motivation system: A theory of avoidance behavior.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):661-675.details
|
|
Keep the scope of neuroethology broad.James A. Simmons - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):400-401.details
|
|
Biological relevance.Howard Rachlin - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):144-144.details
|
|
Where did the notion of “command neurons” come from?Graham Hoyle - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (1):10-11.details
|
|
Must neural mechanisms be Newtonian?Erich Harth - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):550-551.details
|
|
A practical approach to understanding central pattern generators.C. R. S. Kaneko - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):554-554.details
|
|
Pessimism, models, and episodic behavior.James L. Larimer & Wesley Thompson - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):554-555.details
|
|
At what level will pattern generators be understood?V. W. Pentreath - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):559-559.details
|
|
Are central pattern generators understandable?Allen I. Selverston - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):535-540.details
|
|
Epigenesis and phylogenesis: Re-ordering the priorities.Timothy D. Johnston & Gilbert Gottlieb - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):243-244.details
|
|
The search for an alternative to the sociobiological hypothesis.Peter J. Richerson & Robert T. Boyd - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):248-249.details
|
|
The leveller no. 1: Evolution, development, and culture.Mark Ridley - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):249-250.details
|
|
Perceptions and learning in self-control.Robert Eisenberger - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):682-683.details
|
|
Précis of Gallistel's The organization of action: A new synthesis.C. R. Gallistel - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):609-619.details
|
|
Challenges to an interactionist approach to the study of song development.Timothy D. Johnston - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):651-663.details
|
|
In defense of innateness and of its critics.Jonathan Schull - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):646-647.details
|
|
The nature/nurture debate: Same old wolf in new sheep's clothing?Horst D. Steklis - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):649-650.details
|
|
“Template theory” is heuristic in disentangling organism–environment interactions.Hans-Rudolf Güttinger - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):634-635.details
|
|
Behavior-genetic analysis versus ontogenetic imperialism.Jerry Hirsch - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):635-636.details
|
|
Selective breeding–selective rearing interactions and the ontogeny of aggressive behavior.Kathryn E. Hood - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):636-636.details
|
|
The use of evolutionary analogies and the rejection of state variables by B. F. Skinner.Alejandro Kacelnik & Alasdair Houston - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):691-692.details
|
|
Cost–benefit models and the evolution of behavior.Jerram L. Brown - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):682-682.details
|
|
Consequence contingencies and provenance partitions.Juan D. Delius - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):685-685.details
|
|