Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Liberal Representationalism: A Deflationist Defense.Marc Artiga - 2016 - Dialectica 70 (3):407-430.
    The idea that only complex brains can possess genuine representations is an important element in mainstream philosophical thinking. An alternative view, which I label ‘liberal representationalism’, holds that we should accept the existence of many more full-blown representations, from activity in retinal ganglion cells to the neural states produced by innate releasing mechanisms in cognitively unsophisticated organisms. A promising way of supporting liberal representationalism is to show it to be a consequence of our best naturalistic theories of representation. However, several (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • The ontogenesis of narrative: from moving to meaning.Jonathan T. Delafield-Butt & Colwyn Trevarthen - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • A teleofunctional account of evolutionary mismatch.Nathan Cofnas - 2016 - Biology and Philosophy 31 (4):507-525.
    When the environment in which an organism lives deviates in some essential way from that to which it is adapted, this is described as “evolutionary mismatch,” or “evolutionary novelty.” The notion of mismatch plays an important role, explicitly or implicitly, in evolution-informed cognitive psychology, clinical psychology, and medicine. The evolutionary novelty of our contemporary environment is thought to have significant implications for our health and well-being. However, scientists have generally been working without a clear definition of mismatch. This paper defines (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Gricean Communication, Joint Action, and the Evolution of Cooperation.Richard Moore - 2018 - Topoi 37 (2):329-341.
    It is sometimes claimed that Gricean communication is necessarily a form of cooperative or ‘joint’ action. A consequence of this Cooperative Communication View is that Gricean communication could not itself contribute to an explanation of the possibility of joint action. I argue that even though Gricean communication is often a form of joint action, it is not necessarily so—since it does not always require intentional action on the part of a hearer. Rejecting the Cooperative Communication View has attractive consequences for (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Darwin’s Rehabilitation of Teleology Versus Williams’ Replacement of Teleology by Natural Selection.Harry Smit - 2010 - Biological Theory 5 (4):357-365.
    Williams argued that Darwin replaced teleology by natural selection. This article argues that this idea is based on a misunderstanding of Darwin’s critique of the argument from design. Darwin did not replace teleology by evolutionary explanations but showed that we can understand teleology without referring to a Designer. He eliminated the concept of design and rehabilitated Aristotelian teleological explanations. The implication is that adaptations should not be investigated as if designed, but with the help of both teleological and evolutionary explanations. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Can a sociobiology of mind discard the will?Ian Vine - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):318-319.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Toward an empirical foundation for evolutionary psychology.David M. Buss - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):301-302.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Genetically determined neural modules versus mental constructional acts in the genesis of human intelligence.Kathleen R. Gibson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):308-309.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Calls as labels: An intriguing theme, but one with limitations.Donald H. Owings - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):162-163.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Knowing thyself, knowing the other: They're not the same.Jonathan Schull & J. David Smith - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):166-167.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Monkeys and consciousness.D. M. Armstrong - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):147-148.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cerebellar contributions to response selection.Gary G. Berntson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):214-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The mobility gradient: Useful, general, falsifiable?John A. Byers - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):270-271.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Eshkol-Wachman movement notation and the evolution of locomotor patterns in vertebrates.Robert C. Eaton - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):272-274.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Motives: Metaphors in motion.John C. Fentress - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):219-219.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The natural geometry of a behavioral homology.Ilan Golani - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):291-308.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What are voluntary movements made of?Ian Q. Whishaw - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):290-291.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Tentative analysis of apomorphine-induced intraspecific aggressive behavior in the rat according to Adams's classification.B. Senault - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):226-227.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning theory in its niche.Howard Rachlin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):155-156.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Adaptive modification of behavior: Processing information from the environment.Wolfgang M. Schleidt - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):158-159.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • An ecological approach toward a unified theory of learning.William R. Charlesworth - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):142-143.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The relevance of phylogenetics to the study of behavioral diversity.Michael T. Ghiselin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):144-145.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The ecological approach to learning.John Kruse & Edward Reed - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):148-149.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Learning theory: Behavioral artifacts or general principles?John A. Nevin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):152-153.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • EEG desynchronization is associated with cellular events that are prerequisites for active behavioral states.M. Steriade - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):489-492.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A theory in need of defense.Marc N. Branch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):678-679.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Avoidance is in the head, not the genes.Everett J. Wyers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):685-685.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The defense motivation system: A theory of avoidance behavior.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):661-675.
    A motivational system approach to avoidance behavior is presented. According to this approach, a motivational state increases the probability of relevant response patterns and establishes the appropriate or “ideal” consummatory stimuli as positive reinforcers. In the case of feeding motivation, for example, hungry rats are likely to explore and gnaw, and to learn to persist in activities correlated with the reception of consummatory stimuli produced by ingestion of palatable substances. In the case of defense motivation, fearful rats are likely to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Keep the scope of neuroethology broad.James A. Simmons - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):400-401.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Biological relevance.Howard Rachlin - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):144-144.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Where did the notion of “command neurons” come from?Graham Hoyle - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (1):10-11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Must neural mechanisms be Newtonian?Erich Harth - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):550-551.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A practical approach to understanding central pattern generators.C. R. S. Kaneko - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):554-554.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pessimism, models, and episodic behavior.James L. Larimer & Wesley Thompson - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):554-555.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • At what level will pattern generators be understood?V. W. Pentreath - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):559-559.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are central pattern generators understandable?Allen I. Selverston - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):535-540.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   129 citations  
  • Epigenesis and phylogenesis: Re-ordering the priorities.Timothy D. Johnston & Gilbert Gottlieb - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):243-244.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • The search for an alternative to the sociobiological hypothesis.Peter J. Richerson & Robert T. Boyd - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):248-249.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • The leveller no. 1: Evolution, development, and culture.Mark Ridley - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):249-250.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Perceptions and learning in self-control.Robert Eisenberger - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):682-683.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Précis of Gallistel's The organization of action: A new synthesis.C. R. Gallistel - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):609-619.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   64 citations  
  • Challenges to an interactionist approach to the study of song development.Timothy D. Johnston - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):651-663.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • In defense of innateness and of its critics.Jonathan Schull - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):646-647.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The nature/nurture debate: Same old wolf in new sheep's clothing?Horst D. Steklis - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):649-650.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • “Template theory” is heuristic in disentangling organism–environment interactions.Hans-Rudolf Güttinger - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):634-635.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Behavior-genetic analysis versus ontogenetic imperialism.Jerry Hirsch - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):635-636.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Selective breeding–selective rearing interactions and the ontogeny of aggressive behavior.Kathryn E. Hood - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):636-636.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The use of evolutionary analogies and the rejection of state variables by B. F. Skinner.Alejandro Kacelnik & Alasdair Houston - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):691-692.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cost–benefit models and the evolution of behavior.Jerram L. Brown - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):682-682.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Consequence contingencies and provenance partitions.Juan D. Delius - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):685-685.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark