Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Balancing energetic and cognitive resources: Memory use during search depends on the orienting effector.Grayden J. F. Solman & Alan Kingstone - 2014 - Cognition 132 (3):443-454.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • A psychologically-based taxonomy of misdirection.Gustav Kuhn, Hugo A. Caffaratti, Robert Teszka & Ronald A. Rensink - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Magicians use misdirection to prevent you from realizing the methods used to create a magical effect, thereby allowing you to experience an apparently impossible event. Magicians have acquired much knowledge about misdirection, and have suggested several taxonomies of misdirection. These describe many of the fundamental principles in misdirection, focusing on how misdirection is achieved by magicians. In this article we review the strengths and weaknesses of past taxonomies, and argue that a more natural way of making sense of misdirection is (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • The dual function of social gaze.Matthias S. Gobel, Heejung S. Kim & Daniel C. Richardson - 2015 - Cognition 136 (C):359-364.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Computational modelling of visual attention.L. Itti & C. Koch - 2001 - Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2 (3):194-203.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   100 citations  
  • When good observers go bad: Change blindness, inattentional blindness, and visual experience.Ronald A. Rensink - 2000 - PSYCHE: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research On Consciousness 6 (9).
    Several studies (e.g., Becklen & Cervone, 1983; Mack & Rock, 1998; Neisser & Becklen, 1975) have found that observers attending to a particular object or event often fail to report the presence of unexpected items. This has been interpreted as inattentional blindness (IB), a failure to see unattended items (Mack & Rock, 1998). Meanwhile, other studies (e.g., Pashler, 1988; Phillips, 1974; Rensink et al., 1997; Simons, 1996) have found that observers often fail to report the presence of large changes in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes.Ronald A. Rensink, J. Kevin O'Regan & James J. Clark - 1997 - Psychological Science 8:368-373.
    When looking at a scene, observers feel that they see its entire structure in great detail and can immediately notice any changes in it. However, when brief blank fields are placed between alternating displays of an original and a modified scene, a striking failure of perception is induced: identification of changes becomes extremely difficult, even when changes are large and made repeatedly. Identification is much faster when a verbal cue is provided, showing that poor visibility is not the cause of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   194 citations  
  • The Premotor theory of attention: time to move on?Daniel T. Smith & Thomas Schenk - 2012 - Neuropsychologia 50 (6):1104-14.
    Spatial attention and eye-movements are tightly coupled, but the precise nature of this coupling is controversial. The influential but controversial Premotor theory of attention makes four specific predictions about the relationship between motor preparation and spatial attention. Firstly, spatial attention and motor preparation use the same neural substrates. Secondly, spatial attention is functionally equivalent to planning goal directed actions such as eye-movements (i.e. planning an action is both necessary and sufficient for a shift of spatial attention). Thirdly, planning a goal (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Orienting of attention.M. I. Posner - 1980 - Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 32 (1):3-25.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   426 citations