Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Perceptual symbol systems.Lawrence W. Barsalou - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (4):577-660.
    Prior to the twentieth century, theories of knowledge were inherently perceptual. Since then, developments in logic, statis- tics, and programming languages have inspired amodal theories that rest on principles fundamentally different from those underlying perception. In addition, perceptual approaches have become widely viewed as untenable because they are assumed to implement record- ing systems, not conceptual systems. A perceptual theory of knowledge is developed here in the context of current cognitive science and neuroscience. During perceptual experience, association areas in the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   737 citations  
  • Brain embodiment of category-specific semantic memory circuits.L. Boroditsky & J. Prinz - 2008 - In Gün R. Semin & Eliot R. Smith (eds.), Embodied grounding: social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Asymmetric neural control systems in human self-regulation.Don M. Tucker & Peter A. Williamson - 1984 - Psychological Review 91 (2):185-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   85 citations  
  • On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case.Michael C. Anderson & Barbara A. Spellman - 1995 - Psychological Review 102 (1):68-100.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Temporal construal.Yaacov Trope & Nira Liberman - 2003 - Psychological Review 110 (3):403-421.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   106 citations  
  • Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors.Lera Boroditsky - 2000 - Cognition 75 (1):1-28.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   167 citations  
  • Embodied grounding: social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches.Gün R. Semin & Eliot R. Smith (eds.) - 2008 - New York: Cambridge University Press.
    In recent years there has been an increasing awareness that a comprehensive understanding of language, cognitive and affective processes, and social and interpersonal phenomena cannot be achieved without understanding the ways these processes are grounded in bodily states. The term ‘embodiment’ captures the common denominator of these developments, which come from several disciplinary perspectives ranging from neuroscience, cognitive science, social psychology, and affective sciences. For the first time, this volume brings together these varied developments under one umbrella and furnishes a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Visuospatial reasoning.Barbara Tversky - 2005 - In K. Holyoak & B. Morrison (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. pp. 209--240.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • The embodiment of power and communalism in space and bodily contact.Thomas W. Schubert, Sven Waldzus & Beate Seibt - 2008 - In Gün R. Semin & Eliot R. Smith (eds.), Embodied grounding: social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 160--183.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Time in the mind: Using space to think about time.Daniel Casasanto & Lera Boroditsky - 2008 - Cognition 106 (2):579-593.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   118 citations  
  • On the Experiential Link Between Spatial and Temporal Language.Teenie Matlock, Michael Ramscar & Lera Boroditsky - 2005 - Cognitive Science 29 (4):655-664.
    How do we understand time and other entities we can neither touch nor see? One possibility is that we tap into our concrete, experiential knowledge, including our understanding of physical space and motion, to make sense of abstract domains such as time. To examine how pervasive an aspect of cognition this is, we investigated whether thought about a nonliteral type of motion called fictive motion (FM; as in The road runs along the coast) can influence thought about time. Our results (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Levels of personal agency: individual variation in action identification.Robin R. Vallacher & Daniel M. Wegner - unknown
    This research examined individual differences in action identification level as measured by the Behavior Identification Form. Action identification theory holds that any action can be identified in many ways, ranging from low-level identities that specify how the action is performed to high-level identities that signify why or with what effect the action performed. People who identify action at a uniformly lower or higher level across many action domains, then, may be characterized in terms of their standing on a broad personality (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   50 citations  
  • How Mental Systems Believe.Daniel T. Gilbert - 1991 - American Psychologist 46 (2):107-119.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   110 citations  
  • Embodiment of social cognition and relationships.G. R. Semin & J. T. Cacioppo - 2008 - In Gün R. Semin & Eliot R. Smith (eds.), Embodied grounding: social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Narratives of space, time, and life.Barbara Tversky - 2004 - Mind and Language 19 (4):380–392.
    The mind constructs narratives from what would otherwise be chaos. Narratives viewed minimally—at least two temporally ordered events—are revealed in the way people talk about space and time. Narratives replete with a voice, causality, and emotion are reflected in the stories people tell about their own lives, stories that, as acknowledged by their tellers, distort the details around 60% of the time, but, according to their tellers, distort the 'truth' far less often.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations