Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. On saying that.Donald Davidson - 1968 - Synthese 19 (1-2):130-146.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   133 citations  
  • Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states?Stephen Andrew Butterfill & Ian A. Apperly - 2009 - Psychological Review 116 (4):953-970.
    The lack of consensus on how to characterize humans’ capacity for belief reasoning has been brought into sharp focus by recent research. Children fail critical tests of belief reasoning before 3 to 4 years (Wellman, Cross, & Watson, 2001; Wimmer & Perner, 1983), yet infants apparently pass false belief tasks at 13 or 15 months (Onishi & Baillargeon, 2005; Surian, Caldi, & Sperber, 2007). Non-human animals also fail critical tests of belief reasoning but can show very complex social behaviour (e.g., (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   251 citations  
  • Addressees distinguish shared from private information when interpreting questions during interactive conversation.Sarah Brown-Schmidt, Christine Gunlogson & Michael K. Tanenhaus - 2008 - Cognition 107 (3):1122-1134.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • Partner‐Specific Adaptation in Dialog.Susan E. Brennan & Joy E. Hanna - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (2):274-291.
    No one denies that people adapt what they say and how they interpret what is said to them, depending on their interactive partners. What is controversial is when and how they do so. Several psycholinguistics research programs have found what appear to be failures to adapt to partners in the early moments of processing and have used this evidence to argue for modularity in the language processing architecture, claiming that the system cannot take into account a partner’s distinct needs or (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Pragmatic expectations and linguistic evidence: Listeners anticipate but do not integrate common ground.Dale J. Barr - 2008 - Cognition 109 (1):18-40.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Opacity and discourse referents: Object identity and object properties.Manuel Sprung, Josef Perner & Peter Mitchell - 2007 - Mind and Language 22 (3):215–245.
    It has been found that children appreciate the limited substitutability of co-referential terms in opaque contexts a year or two after they pass false belief tasks (e.g. Apperly and Robinson, 1998, 2001, 2003). This paper aims to explain this delay. Three- to six-year-old children were tested with stories where a protagonist was either only partially informed or had a false belief about a particular object. Only a few children had problems predicting the protagonist’s action based on his partial knowledge, when (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Addressees distinguish shared from private information when interpreting questions during interactive conversation.Michael K. Tanenhaus Sarah Brown-Schmidt, Christine Gunlogson - 2008 - Cognition 107 (3):1122.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Transparent and opaque reference.Robert Ray - 1980 - Philosophical Studies 38 (4):435 - 445.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue.Martin J. Pickering & Simon Garrod - 2004 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (2):169-190.
    Traditional mechanistic accounts of language processing derive almost entirely from the study of monologue. Yet, the most natural and basic form of language use is dialogue. As a result, these accounts may only offer limited theories of the mechanisms that underlie language processing in general. We propose a mechanistic account of dialogue, the interactive alignment account, and use it to derive a number of predictions about basic language processes. The account assumes that, in dialogue, the linguistic representations employed by the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   258 citations  
  • Reflexively mindblind: Using theory of mind to interpret behavior requires effortful attention.Shuhong Lin, Boaz Keysar & Nicholas Epley - 2010 - Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46 (3):551-556.
    People commonly interpret others’ behavior in terms of the actors’ underlying beliefs, knowledge, or other mental states, thereby using their “theory of mind.” Two experiments suggest that using one’s theory of mind is a relatively effortful process. In both experiments, people reflexively used their own knowledge and beliefs to follow a speaker’s instruction, but only effortfully used their theory of mind to take into account a speaker’s intention to interpret those instructions. In Experiment 1, people with lower working memory capacity (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Limits on theory of mind use in adults.Boaz Keysar, Shuhong Lin & Dale J. Barr - 2003 - Cognition 89 (1):25-41.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   111 citations  
  • Six-year-olds' difficulties handling intensional contexts.Sarah Hulme, Peter Mitchell & David Wood - 2003 - Cognition 87 (2):73-99.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • The role of perspective in identifying domains of reference.Daphna Heller, Daniel Grodner & Michael K. Tanenhaus - 2008 - Cognition 108 (3):831-836.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Pragmatic effects on reference resolution in a collaborative task: evidence from eye movements.Joy E. Hanna & Michael K. Tanenhaus - 2004 - Cognitive Science 28 (1):105-115.
    In order to investigate whether addressees can make immediate use of speaker‐based constraints during reference resolution, participant addressees' eye movements were monitored as they helped a confederate cook follow a recipe. Objects were located in the helper's area, which the cook could not reach, and the cook's area, which both could reach. Critical referring expressions matched one object (helper's area) or two objects (helper's and cook's areas), and were produced when the cook's hands were empty or full, which defined the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  • Insides and Essences: Early Understandings of the Non- Obvious.Susan A. Gelman & Henry M. Wellman - 1991 - Cognition 38 (3):213-244.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   180 citations  
  • Categories and induction in young children.Susan A. Gelman & Ellen M. Markman - 1986 - Cognition 23 (3):183-209.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   219 citations  
  • Eye movements reveal the time-course of anticipating behaviour based on complex, conflicting desires.Heather J. Ferguson & Richard Breheny - 2011 - Cognition 119 (2):179-196.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • The Egocentric Basis of Language Use: Insights from a Processing Approach.Boaz Keysar, Dale Barr, Horton J. & S. William - 1998 - Current Directions in Psychological Sciences 7:46--50.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Grounding in communication.Herbert H. Clark & Susan E. Brennan - 1991 - In Lauren Resnick, Levine B., M. John, Stephanie Teasley & D. (eds.), Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition. American Psychological Association. pp. 13--1991.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   76 citations