Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Artificial cognition for social human–robot interaction: An implementation.Séverin Lemaignan, Mathieu Warnier, E. Akin Sisbot, Aurélie Clodic & Rachid Alami - 2017 - Artificial Intelligence 247:45-69.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Anthropomorphism: Opportunities and Challenges in Human-Robot Interaction.Jakub Zlotowski, Diane Proudfoot, Kumar Yogeeswaran & Christoph Bartneck - 2015 - International Journal of Social Robotics 7 (3):347-360.
    Anthropomorphism is a phenomenon that describes the human tendency to see human-like shapes in the environment. It has considerable consequences for people’s choices and beliefs. With the increased presence of robots, it is important to investigate the optimal design for this tech- nology. In this paper we discuss the potential benefits and challenges of building anthropomorphic robots, from both a philosophical perspective and from the viewpoint of empir- ical research in the fields of human–robot interaction and social psychology. We believe (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • The role of beliefs in lexical alignment: Evidence from dialogs with humans and computers.Holly P. Branigan, Martin J. Pickering, Jamie Pearson, Janet F. McLean & Ash Brown - 2011 - Cognition 121 (1):41-57.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Referring as a collaborative process.Herbert H. Clark & Deanna Wilkes-Gibbs - 1986 - Cognition 22 (1):1-39.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   194 citations  
  • Conceptualizing agency: Folkpsychological and folkcommunicative perspectives on plants.Bethany L. Ojalehto, Douglas L. Medin & Salino G. García - 2017 - Cognition 162 (C):103-123.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The mind in the machine: Anthropomorphism increases trust in an autonomous vehicle.Adam Waytz, Joy Heafner & Nicholas Epley - 2014 - Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 52:113-117.
    Sophisticated technology is increasingly replacing human minds to perform complicated tasks in domains ranging from medicine to education to transportation. We investigated an important theoretical determinant of people's willingness to trust such technology to perform competently—the extent to which a nonhuman agent is anthropomorphized with a humanlike mind—in a domain of practical importance, autonomous driving. Participants using a driving simulator drove either a normal car, an autonomous vehicle able to control steering and speed, or a comparable autonomous vehicle augmented with (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Strategies for Human‐Driven Robot Comprehension of Spatial Descriptions by Older Adults in a Robot Fetch Task.Laura Carlson, Marjorie Skubic, Jared Miller, Zhiyu Huo & Tatiana Alexenko - 2014 - Topics in Cognitive Science 6 (3):513-533.
    This contribution presents a corpus of spatial descriptions and describes the development of a human-driven spatial language robot system for their comprehension. The domain of application is an eldercare setting in which an assistive robot is asked to “fetch” an object for an elderly resident based on a natural language spatial description given by the resident. In Part One, we describe a corpus of naturally occurring descriptions elicited from a group of older adults within a virtual 3D home that simulates (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking.Barbara Tversky & Bridgette Martin Hard - 2009 - Cognition 110 (1):124-129.
    Although people can take spatial perspectives different from their own, it is widely assumed that egocentric perspectives are natural and have primacy. Two studies asked respondents to describe the spatial relations between two objects on a table in photographed scenes; in some versions, a person sitting behind the objects was either looking at or reaching for one of the objects. The mere presence of another person in a position to act on the objects induced a good proportion of respondents to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  • Toward Integrative Dynamic Models for Adaptive Perspective Taking.Nicholas Duran, Rick Dale & Alexia Galati - 2016 - Topics in Cognitive Science 8 (4):761-779.
    In a matter of mere milliseconds, conversational partners can transform their expectations about the world in a way that accords with another person's perspective. At the same time, in similar situations, the exact opposite also appears to be true. Rather than being at odds, these findings suggest that there are multiple contextual and processing constraints that may guide when and how people consider perspective. These constraints are shaped by a host of factors, including the availability of social and environmental cues, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Who sees human? The stability and importance of individual differences in anthropomorphism.Adam Waytz, John Cacioppo & Nicholas Epley - 2010 - Perspectives on Psychological Science 5 (3):219-232.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Social and Representational Cues Jointly Influence Spatial Perspective‐Taking.Alexia Galati & Marios N. Avraamides - 2015 - Cognitive Science 39 (4):739-765.
    We examined how social cues and representational ones jointly shape people's spatial memory representations and their subsequent descriptions. In 24 pairs, Directors studied an array with a symmetrical structure while either knowing their Matcher's subsequent viewpoint or not. During the subsequent description of the array, the array's intrinsic structure was aligned with the Director, the Matcher, or neither partner. According to memory tests preceding descriptions, Directors who had studied the array while aligned with its structure were more likely to use (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Interacting Timescales in Perspective-Taking.Rick Dale, Alexia Galati, Camila Alviar, Pablo Contreras Kallens, Adolfo G. Ramirez-Aristizabal, Maryam Tabatabaeian & David W. Vinson - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:320582.
    Through theoretical discussion, literature review, and a computational model, this paper poses a challenge to the notion that perspective-taking involves a fixed architecture in which particular processes have priority. For example, some research suggests that egocentric perspectives can arise more quickly, with other perspectives (such as of task partners) emerging only secondarily. This theoretical dichotomy–between fast egocentric and slow other-centric processes–is challenged here. We propose a general view of perspective-taking as an emergent phenomenon governed by the interplay among cognitive mechanisms (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Spatial perspective-taking in conversation.Michael F. Schober - 1993 - Cognition 47 (1):1-24.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • (S)he's Got the Look: Gender Stereotyping of Robots 1.Friederike Eyssel & Frank Hegel - 2012 - Journal of Applied Social Psychology 42 (9):2213-2230.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Listeners invest in an assumed other’s perspective despite cognitive cost.Nicholas D. Duran, Rick Dale & Roger J. Kreuz - 2011 - Cognition 121 (1):22-40.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations