Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Eliminate the middletoad!Daniel Dennett - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):372-374.
    Philosophical controversy about the mind has flourished in the thin air of our ignorance about the brain. The humble toad, it now seems, may provide our first instance of a creature whose whole brain is within the reach of our scientific understanding. What will happen to the traditional philosophical issues as our theoretical and factual ignorance recedes? Discussion of the issues explored in the target article is, as Ewert says, "often too theoretical, sometimes philosophical and even [as if that weren't (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • The lambda model is only one piece in the motor control puzzle.Jeffrey Dean - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):749-749.
    The lambda model provides a physiologically grounded terminology for describing muscle function and emphasizes the important influence of environmental and reflex-mediated effects on final states. However, lambda itself is only a convenient point on the length-tension curve; its importance should not be overemphasized. Ascribing movement to changes in a lambda-based frame of reference is generally valid, but it leaves unanswered a number of questions concerning mechanisms.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sleep cycle or REM sleep generator?Serge Daan, Domien G. M. Beersma & Derk Jan Dijk - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):402-403.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Successive approximation in targeted movement: An alternative hypothesis.Paul J. Cordo & Leslie Bevan - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):729-730.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On to real-life movements.Paul J. Cordo, Fay B. Horak & Susan P. Moore - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):214-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does constraining movements constrain the developement of movement theories?Daniel M. Corcos, Gerland L. Gottlieb & Gyan C. Agarwal - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):237-250.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Conservative or nonconservative control schemes.Daniel M. Corcos & Kerstin Pfann - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):747-749.
    The conservative strategy proposed by the authors suggests a solution of the degrees-of-freedom problem of the controller. However, several simple motor control tasks cannot be explained by this strategy. A nonconservative strategy, in which more parameters of the control signal vary, can account for these simple motor tasks. However, the simplicity that distinguishes the proposed model from many others is lost.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sensorimotor functions: What is a command, that a code may yield it?Christopher M. Comer - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):372-372.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Development of Infant Reaching Strategies to Tactile Targets on the Face.Lisa K. Chinn, Claire F. Noonan, Matej Hoffmann & Jeffrey J. Lockman - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Four correlates of complex behavioral networks: Differentiation, behavior, connectivity, and compartmentalization: Carving networks at their joints.Mark A. Changizi & Darren He - 2005 - Complexity 10 (6):13-40.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Relationships between pontogeniculooccipital waves and ocular movements.Raymond Cespuglio - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):401-402.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How is a toad not like a bug?Jeffrey M. Camhi - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):371-372.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Strategies and motor programs.Bruce D. Burns & Jeffery J. Summers - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):214-214.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Saturation is not an evolutlonarily stable strategy.Daniel Bullock - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):212-214.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • After the sensory analysers: Problems with concepts and terminology.D. M. Broom - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):370-371.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Skeletal and oculomotor control systems compared.Bruce Bridgeman - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):212-212.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the importance of individual differences in hypnotic ability.Kenneth S. Bowers & Thomas M. Davidson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):468-469.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sleep homeostasis.Alexander A. Borbély - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):401-401.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A few reasons why psychologlsts can adhere to Feldman and Levin's model.Mireille Bonnard & Jean Pailhous - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):746-747.
    We emphasize the relevance to cognitive psychology of Feldman and Levin's theoretical position. Traditional views of motor control have failed to clearly separate “production control” at the level of motor command, based on task-independent CV (control variables), from intentional “product control” based on task-dependent parameters. Because F&L's approach concentrates on the first process (trajectory formation), it can distinguish the product control stage.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does the nervous system use equilibrium-point control to guide single and multiple joint movements?E. Bizzi, N. Hogan, F. A. Mussa-Ivaldi & S. Giszter - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):603-613.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  • The organization and optimization of movement.M. B. Berkinblit, A. G. Feldman & O. I. Fukson - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):719-720.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The centrality of instantiations.John A. Barnden - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):437-438.
    This paper is a commentary on the target article by Michael Arbib, “Levels of modeling of mechanisms of visually guided behavior”, in the same issue of the journal, pp. 407–465. -/- I focus on the importance of the inclusion of an ability of a system to entertain, at a given time, multiple instantiations of a given schema (situation template, frame, script, action plan, etc.), and complications introduced into neural/connectionist network systems by such inclusion.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Biologically applied neural networks may foster the coevolution of neurobiology and Cognitive psychology.Bill Baird - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):436-437.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ethology and physiology: A happy marriage.Gerard P. Baerends - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):369-370.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Of schemas, neural nets, and Rana computatrix.Michael A. Arbib - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):451-465.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Levels of modeling of mechanisms of visually guided behavior.Michael A. Arbib - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):407-436.
    Intermediate constructs are required as bridges between complex behaviors and realistic models of neural circuitry. For cognitive scientists in general, schemas are the appropriate functional units; brain theorists can work with neural layers as units intermediate between structures subserving schemas and small neural circuits.After an account of different levels of analysis, we describe visuomotor coordination in terms of perceptual schemas and motor schemas. The interest of schemas to cognitive science in general is illustrated with the example of perceptual schemas in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Advantages of experimentation in neuroscience.Michael A. Arbib - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):368-369.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rapid eye movements and the cerebellum.John Antrobus - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):400-401.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Tendon elasticity and positional control.R. McN Alexander - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-745.
    The spring-like behaviour of a joint following a sudden change of torque is partly a result of the elastic properties of tendons. A large fall in a muscle with a long tendon may be accompanied by tendon recoil causing joint movements as large as 20°.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is the multi-joint pointing movement model applicable to equilibrium control during upper trunk movements?Alexey Alexandrov, Alexander Frolov & Jean Massion - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-746.
    Two aspects of the target article, (1) the extension of the equilibrium point theory to multi-joint movements, and (2) the consequence that the EMG pattern is not directly controlled by the central nervous system (CNS), are discussed in light of the experiments on upper trunk bending in humans. The principle component kinematic analysis and the analysis of the EMG data, obtained under microgravity and additional loading conditions, support the application of Feldman and Levin's for multi-joint pointing movement to equilibrium control (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The prerequisites for one-jint motor control theories.S. V. Adamovich & A. G. Feldman - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):210-211.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • How does the nervous system control the equilibrium trajectory?S. V. Adamovich - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):704-705.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • On simple movements and complex theories (and vice versa).K. M. Newell, R. E. A. Van Emmerik & P. V. McDonald - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):229-230.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • On simple movements and complex theories (and vice versa).K. M. Newell, R. E. A. van Emmerik & P. V. McDonald - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):229-230.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Strategies for the control of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom.Gerald L. Gottlieb, Daniel M. Corcos & Gyan C. Agarwal - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):189-210.
    A theory is presented to explain how accurate, single-joint movements are controlled. The theory applies to movements across different distances, with different inertial loads, toward targets of different widths over a wide range of experimentally manipulated velocities. The theory is based on three propositions. (1) Movements are planned according to “strategies” of which there are at least two: a speed-insensitive (SI) and a speed-sensitive (SS) one. (2) These strategies can be equated with sets of rules for performing diverse movement tasks. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   183 citations  
  • Biological variability and control of movements via δλ.Charles E. Wright & Rebecca A. States - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):786-786.
    Three issues related to Feldman and Levin's treatment of biological variability are discussed. We question the usefulness of the indirect component of δλ. We suggest that trade-offs between speed and accuracy in aimed movements support identification of δλ, rather than λ, as a control variable. We take issue with the authors' proposal for resolving redundancy in multi-joint movements, given recent data.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What is adapted in strategy-governed movements?U. Windhorst - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):236-237.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Levers to generate movement.U. Windhorst - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):784-785.
    The following questions are discussed: (1) Who determines the nature of “control variables”? (2) Is the “positional monopoly” healthy? (3) Does a descending command alter reflex threshold alone without eoncomitantly altering stiffness? (4) How does the CNS deal with history-dependent effects? (5) Should we abandon the idea that the CNS controls classical Newtonian variables such as muscle length?
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How far should we extend the equilibrium point (lambda) hypothesis?Jack M. Winters - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):785-786.
    A key feature of the lambda model is the hypothesis of a local spring-like muscle-reflex system defined by a central control variable that has units of position. This is intriguing, especially for a study of postural stability in large-scale systems, but it has limited direct application to skilled everyday movements. If movement is considered as a goal-directed, neuro-optimization problem, however, theavailabilityof lambda-like peripheral models (vs. conventional musculoskeletal models) deserves exploration.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Grasping schemas is (are) difficult.H. T. A. Whiting - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):450-451.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The dynamics of perception and action.William H. Warren - 2006 - Psychological Review 113 (2):358-389.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   63 citations  
  • Why are “strategies’ senstitive? Smoothing the way for raison d'àtre”.John P. Wann, Ian Nimmo-Smith & Alan M. Wing - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):235-236.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Initiating voluntary movements: Wrong theories for the wrong behaviour?Stephen A. Wallace & Douglas L. Weeks - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):233-234.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Elementary conditions for elemental movement strategies.Charles B. Walter - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):234-235.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Schemas and bridging gaps in the behavioral and brain sciences.Johan P. Wagemans - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):449-450.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Schema theory: A new approach?W. von Seelen - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):448-449.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A new role for FTG neurons?Robert P. Vertes - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):425-426.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Self-organization of cognitive performance.Guy C. Van Orden, John G. Holden & Michael T. Turvey - 2003 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 132 (3):331.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   95 citations  
  • Schemata and representational constraints.Cees van Leeuwen - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):448-448.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Reciprocal interaction in sleep cycle control: Description, yes; explanation, no.Paul A. M. van Dongen - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):424-425.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark