Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The Gantt and Eysenck conditioning models for neurosis.Roscoe A. Dykman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):168-169.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is this defense needed?James A. Dinsmoor - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):679-679.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • An emotion-attribution approach to moral behavior: Interfacing cognitive and avoidance theories of moral development.Richard A. Dienstbier - 1975 - Psychological Review 82 (4):299-315.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The anhedonia hypothesis for neuroleptics and operant behaviour.T. J. Crow - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):174-174.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Deep and beautiful. The reward prediction error hypothesis of dopamine.Matteo Colombo - 2014 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 45 (1):57-67.
    According to the reward-prediction error hypothesis of dopamine, the phasic activity of dopaminergic neurons in the midbrain signals a discrepancy between the predicted and currently experienced reward of a particular event. It can be claimed that this hypothesis is deep, elegant and beautiful, representing one of the largest successes of computational neuroscience. This paper examines this claim, making two contributions to existing literature. First, it draws a comprehensive historical account of the main steps that led to the formulation and subsequent (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Apertures, Draw, and Syntax: Remodeling Attention.Brian Bruya - 2010 - In Effortless Attention: A New Perspective in the Cognitive Science of Attention and Action. MIT Press. pp. 219.
    Because psychological studies of attention and cognition are most commonly performed within the strict confines of the laboratory or take cognitively impaired patients as subjects, it is difficult to be sure that resultant models of attention adequately account for the phenomenon of effortless attention. The problem is not only that effortless attention is resistant to laboratory study. A further issue is that because the laboratory is the most common way to approach attention, models resulting from such studies are naturally the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • A theory in need of defense.Marc N. Branch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):678-679.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Bowlby-Ainsworth attachment theory.John Bowlby - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):637-638.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Two questions for a general theory of infantile attachment.Marc H. Bornstein - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):636-637.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Incubation and the relevance of functional CS exposure.T. D. Borkovec - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):168-168.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The nonextinction of fear: operation bootstrap.Robert C. Bolles - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):167-168.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • The motivation and/or reinforcement of avoidance behavior.Robert C. Bolles - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):677-678.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What next? A perceptual-motivational approach to attachment.Dalbir Bindra - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):636-636.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How adaptive behavior is produced: a perceptual-motivational alternative to response reinforcements.Dalbir Bindra - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (1):41-52.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   253 citations  
  • Conditioning theory and neurosis.Dalbir Bindra - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):166-167.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Avoidance theory: Solutions or more problems?Philip J. Bersh - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):676-677.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The behavioral function of dopamine.Richard J. Beninger - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):55-56.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Programmed development.Gerard P. Baerends - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):635-636.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Support for the hypothesis that the actions of dopamine are “not merely motor.”.G. W. Arbuthnott - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):54-55.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Anhedonia: Too much, too soon.Hymie Anisman - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):53-54.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A functional consideration of anatomical connections between the basal ganglia and the thalamus suggests that antipsychotic drugs inhibit the initiation of movement.Sven Ahlenius - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):173-174.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Defense motivational system: Issues of emotion, reinforcement, and neural structure.David Adams - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):675-676.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Passive frame theory: A new synthesis.Ezequiel Morsella, Godwin Christine, Jantz Tiffany, Krieger Stephen & Gazzaley Adam - forthcoming - Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
    Passive frame theory attempts to illuminate what consciousness is, in mechanistic and functional terms; it does not address the “implementation” level of analysis (how neurons instantiate conscious states), an enigma for various disciplines. However, in response to the commentaries, we discuss how our framework provides clues regarding this enigma. In the framework, consciousness is passive albeit essential. Without consciousness, there would not be adaptive skeletomotor action.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Homing in on consciousness in the nervous system: An action-based synthesis.Ezequiel Morsella, Christine A. Godwin, Tiffany K. Jantz, Stephen C. Krieger & Adam Gazzaley - 2016 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39:1-70.
    What is the primary function of consciousness in the nervous system? The answer to this question remains enigmatic, not so much because of a lack of relevant data, but because of the lack of a conceptual framework with which to interpret the data. To this end, we have developed Passive Frame Theory, an internally coherent framework that, from an action-based perspective, synthesizes empirically supported hypotheses from diverse fields of investigation. The theory proposes that the primary function of consciousness is well-circumscribed, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • What and where is the unconditioned (or conditioned) stimulus in the conditioning model of neurosis?Marvin Zuckerman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):187-188.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • New perspectives on conditioning models and incubation theory.Susan Mineka - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):178-178.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Toward an unpdated model of neurosis.J. M. Notterman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):178-179.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The conditioning model of neurosis.H. J. Eysenck - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):155-166.
    The long-term persistence of neurotic symptoms, such as anxiety, poses difficult problems for any psychological theory. An attempt is made to revive the Watson-Mowrer conditioning theory and to avoid the many criticisms directed against it in the past. It is suggested that recent research has produced changes in learning theory that can be used to render this possible. In the first place, the doctrine of equipotentiality has been shown to be wrong, and some such concept as Seligman's “preparedness” is required, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   96 citations  
  • Neurolepsis: Anhedonia or blunting of emotional reactivity?Richard H. Rech - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):72-73.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Neuroleptic-induced anhedonia: Some psychopharmacological implications.Philippe Soubrie - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):76-77.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Neuroleptics and operant behavior: The anhedonia hypothesis.Roy A. Wise - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):39-53.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   144 citations  
  • Reinforcement of avoidance behavior.Arlo K. Myers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):681-682.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The anhedonia hypothesis: Mark III.Roy A. Wise - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):178-186.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The function of phenomenal states: Supramodular interaction theory.Ezequiel Morsella - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (4):1000-1021.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   75 citations  
  • Implications of recent research in conditioning for the conditioning model of neurosis.William S. Terry - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):183-184.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction.Roy A. Wise & Michael A. Bozarth - 1987 - Psychological Review 94 (4):469-492.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   89 citations  
  • Reflections on the conditioning model of neurosi.Michael J. Mahoney - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):174-175.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is there any need for conditioning in Eysenck's conditioning model of neurosis?Jeffrey A. Gray - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):169-171.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Criteria for ruling out sedation as an interpretation of neuroleptic effects.William J. Freed & Ronald F. Zec - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):57-59.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Hedonic arousal, memory, and motivation.Leonard D. Katz - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):60-60.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The defense motivation system: A theory of avoidance behavior.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):661-675.
    A motivational system approach to avoidance behavior is presented. According to this approach, a motivational state increases the probability of relevant response patterns and establishes the appropriate or “ideal” consummatory stimuli as positive reinforcers. In the case of feeding motivation, for example, hungry rats are likely to explore and gnaw, and to learn to persist in activities correlated with the reception of consummatory stimuli produced by ingestion of palatable substances. In the case of defense motivation, fearful rats are likely to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Conditioned alpha fear responses and protection from extinction.S. Soltysik - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):182-183.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Journey into the interior of the organism.Howard Rachlin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):180-181.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The anhedonia hypothesis of neuroleptic drug action: Basic and clinical considerations.Charles B. Nemeroff & Daniel Luttinger - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):70-71.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Hypotheses of neuroleptic action: Levels of progress.Roy A. Wise - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):78-87.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Expecting shock.A. W. Logue - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):680-681.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “Prepared fears” and the theory of conditioning.Wanda Wyrwicka - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):186-186.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Eysenck and the Wolpe theories of neurosis.Joseph Wolpe - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):184-185.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The pleasure in brain substrates of foraging.Jaak Panksepp - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):71-72.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Passive frame theory: A new synthesis.Ezequiel Morsella, Christine A. Godwin, Tiffany K. Jantz, Stephen C. Krieger & Adam Gazzaley - 2016 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39:e199.
    Passive frame theory attempts to illuminate what consciousnessis, in mechanistic and functional terms; it does not address the “implementation” level of analysis (how neurons instantiate conscious states), an enigma for various disciplines. However, in response to the commentaries, we discuss how our framework provides clues regarding this enigma. In the framework, consciousness is passive albeit essential. Without consciousness, there would not be adaptive skeletomotor action.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations