Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Reach tracking reveals dissociable processes underlying cognitive control.Christopher D. Erb, Jeff Moher, David M. Sobel & Joo-Hyun Song - 2016 - Cognition 152 (C):114-126.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Cognitive control in action: Tracking the dynamics of rule switching in 5- to 8-year-olds and adults.Christopher D. Erb, Jeff Moher, Joo-Hyun Song & David M. Sobel - 2017 - Cognition 164 (C):163-173.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Is all mental effort equal? The role of cognitive demand-type on effort avoidance.Jake R. Embrey, Chris Donkin & Ben R. Newell - 2023 - Cognition 236 (C):105440.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Comparing perception of Stroop stimuli in focused versus divided attention paradigms: Evidence for dramatic processing differences.Ami Eidels, James T. Townsend & Daniel Algom - 2010 - Cognition 114 (2):129-150.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Tracking intentionalism and the phenomenology of mental effort.Maria Doulatova - 2019 - Synthese 198 (5):4373-4389.
    Most of us are familiar with the phenomenology of mental effort accompanying cognitively demanding tasks, like focusing on the next chess move or performing lengthy mental arithmetic. In this paper, I argue that phenomenology of mental effort poses a novel counterexample to tracking intentionalism, the view that phenomenal consciousness is a matter of tracking features of one’s environment in a certain way. I argue that an increase in the phenomenology of mental effort does not accompany a change in any of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Facial reactions to emotional stimuli: Automatically controlled emotional responses.Ulf Dimberg, Monika Thunberg & Sara Grunedal - 2002 - Cognition and Emotion 16 (4):449-471.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • Structure and Content in Language Production: A Theory of Frame Constraints in Phonological Speech Errors.Gary S. Dell, Cornell Juliano & Anita Govindjee - 1993 - Cognitive Science 17 (2):149-195.
    Theories of language production propose that utterances are constructed by a mechanism that separates linguistic content from linguistic structure, Linguistic content is retrieved from the mental lexicon, and is then inserted into slots in linguistic structures or frames. Support for this kind of model at the phonological level comes from patterns of phonological speech errors. W present an alternative account of these patterns using a connectionist or parallel distributed proceesing (PDP) model that learns to produce sequences of phonological features. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • Affective priming of semantic categorisation responses.Jan De Houwer, Dirk Hermans, Klaus Rothermund & Dirk Wentura - 2002 - Cognition and Emotion 16 (5):643-666.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Processes Versus Representations: Cognitive Control as Emergent, Yet Componential.Eddy J. Davelaar - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (2):247-252.
    In this commentary, I focus on the difference between processes and representations and how this distinction relates to the question of what is controlled. Despite some views that task switching is a prototypical control process, the analysis concludes that task switching depends on the task goal representation and that control processes are there to prevent goal representations from disintegrating. Over time, these processes become obsolete, leaving behind a representation that automatically controls task performance. The distinction between processes and representations relates (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A model of knowledge activation and insight in problem solving.Matthew A. Cronin - 2004 - Complexity 9 (5):17-24.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mechanisms for the generation and regulation of sequential behaviour.Richard P. Cooper - 2003 - Philosophical Psychology 16 (3):389 – 416.
    A critical aspect of much human behaviour is the generation and regulation of sequential activities. Such behaviour is seen in both naturalistic settings such as routine action and language production and laboratory tasks such as serial recall and many reaction time experiments. There are a variety of computational mechanisms that may support the generation and regulation of sequential behaviours, ranging from those underlying Turing machines to those employed by recurrent connectionist networks. This paper surveys a range of such mechanisms, together (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Cognitive Control: Componential or Emergent?Richard P. Cooper - 2010 - Topics in Cognitive Science 2 (4):598-613.
    The past 25 years have witnessed an increasing awareness of the importance of cognitive control in the regulation of complex behavior. It now sits alongside attention, memory, language, and thinking as a distinct domain within cognitive psychology. At the same time it permeates each of these sibling domains. This introduction reviews recent work on cognitive control in an attempt to provide a context for the fundamental question addressed within this topic: Is cognitive control to be understood as resulting from the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Action Production and Event Perception as Routine Sequential Behaviors.Richard P. Cooper - 2021 - Topics in Cognitive Science 13 (1):63-78.
    Topics in Cognitive Science, Volume 13, Issue 1, Page 63-78, January 2021.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Are Automatic Imitation and Spatial Compatibility Mediated by Different Processes?Richard P. Cooper, Caroline Catmur & Cecilia Heyes - 2013 - Cognitive Science 37 (4):605-630.
    Automatic imitation or “imitative compatibility” is thought to be mediated by the mirror neuron system and to be a laboratory model of the motor mimicry that occurs spontaneously in naturalistic social interaction. Imitative compatibility and spatial compatibility effects are known to depend on different stimulus dimensions—body movement topography and relative spatial position. However, it is not yet clear whether these two types of stimulus–response compatibility effect are mediated by the same or different cognitive processes. We present an interactive activation model (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Connecting Conscious and Unconscious Processing.Axel Cleeremans - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (6):1286-1315.
    Consciousness remains a mystery—“a phenomenon that people do not know how to think about—yet” (Dennett, , p. 21). Here, I consider how the connectionist perspective on information processing may help us progress toward the goal of understanding the computational principles through which conscious and unconscious processing differ. I begin by delineating the conceptual challenges associated with classical approaches to cognition insofar as understanding unconscious information processing is concerned, and to highlight several contrasting computational principles that are constitutive of the connectionist (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Competition and cooperation among similar representations: Toward a unified account of facilitative and inhibitory effects of lexical neighbors.Qi Chen & Daniel Mirman - 2012 - Psychological Review 119 (2):417-430.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Understanding covert recognition.A. Mike Burton, Andrew W. Young, Vicki Bruce, Robert A. Johnston & Andrew W. Ellis - 1991 - Cognition 39 (2):129-166.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • In Support of a Distinction between Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Control: A Review of the Literature on Proportion Congruent Effects. [REVIEW]Julie M. Bugg & Matthew J. C. Crump - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • eStroop: Implementation, Standardization, and Systematic Comparison of a New Voice-Key Version of the Traditional Stroop Task.Riccardo Brunetti, Allegra Indraccolo, Claudia Del Gatto, Benedetto Farina, Claudio Imperatori, Elena Fontana, Jacopo Penso, Rita B. Ardito & Mauro Adenzato - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    The Stroop effect is a well-documented phenomenon, demonstrating both interference and facilitation effects. Many versions of the Stroop task were created, according to the purposes of its applications, varying in numerous aspects. While many versions are developed to investigate the mechanisms of the effect itself, the Stroop effect is also considered a general measure of attention, inhibitory control, and executive functions. In this paper, we implement “eStroop”: a new digital version based on verbal responses, measuring the main processes involved in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Computational and Neural Basis of Cognitive Control: Charted Territory and New Frontiers.Matthew M. Botvinick - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (6):1249-1285.
    Cognitive control has long been one of the most active areas of computational modeling work in cognitive science. The focus on computational models as a medium for specifying and developing theory predates the PDP books, and cognitive control was not one of the areas on which they focused. However, the framework they provided has injected work on cognitive control with new energy and new ideas. On the occasion of the books' anniversary, we review computational modeling in the study of cognitive (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: A reinforcement learning perspective.Matthew M. Botvinick, Yael Niv & Andrew C. Barto - 2009 - Cognition 113 (3):262-280.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: A reinforcement learning perspective.Matthew M. Botvinick, Yael Niv & Andew G. Barto - 2009 - Cognition 113 (3):262-280.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • Doing Without Schema Hierarchies: A Recurrent Connectionist Approach to Normal and Impaired Routine Sequential Action.Matthew Botvinick & David C. Plaut - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (2):395-429.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   53 citations  
  • Conflict monitoring and cognitive control.Matthew M. Botvinick, Todd S. Braver, Deanna M. Barch, Cameron S. Carter & Jonathan D. Cohen - 2001 - Psychological Review 108 (3):624-652.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   431 citations  
  • The physics of optimal decision making: A formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks.Rafal Bogacz, Eric Brown, Jeff Moehlis, Philip Holmes & Jonathan D. Cohen - 2006 - Psychological Review 113 (4):700-765.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   73 citations  
  • The Stroop Effect Occurs at Multiple Points Along a Cascade of Control: Evidence From Cognitive Neuroscience Approaches.Marie T. Banich - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Defining an Ontology of Cognitive Control Requires Attention to Component Interactions.David Badre - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (2):217-221.
    Cognitive control is not only componential, but those components may interact in complicated ways in the service of cognitive control tasks. This complexity poses a challenge for developing an ontological description, because the mapping may not be direct between our task descriptions and true component differences reflected in indicators. To illustrate this point, I discuss two examples: (a) the relationship between adaptive gating and working memory and (b) the recent evidence for a control hierarchy. From these examples, I argue that (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The Loci of Stroop Interference and Facilitation Effects With Manual and Vocal Responses.Maria Augustinova, Benjamin A. Parris & Ludovic Ferrand - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Explicatures are NOT Cancellable.Alessandro Capone - 2013 - In Alessandro Capone, Franco Lo Piparo & Marco Carapezza (eds.), Perspectives on linguistic pragmatics. Springer. pp. 131-151.
    Explicatures are not cancellable. Theoretical considerations.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Principles for Implicit Learning.Axel Cleeremans - 1997 - In Dianne C. Berry (ed.), How Implicit is Implicit Learning? Oxford University Press.
    Complete URL to this document: http://srsc.ulb.ac.be/axcWWW/93-Principles.html.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Can resources save rationality? ‘Anti-Bayesian’ updating in cognition and perception.Eric Mandelbaum, Isabel Won, Steven Gross & Chaz Firestone - 2020 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 143:e16.
    Resource rationality may explain suboptimal patterns of reasoning; but what of “anti-Bayesian” effects where the mind updates in a direction opposite the one it should? We present two phenomena — belief polarization and the size-weight illusion — that are not obviously explained by performance- or resource-based constraints, nor by the authors’ brief discussion of reference repulsion. Can resource rationality accommodate them?
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Neurodemocracy: Self-Organization of the Embodied Mind.Linus Huang - 2017 - Dissertation, University of Sydney
    This thesis contributes to a better conceptual understanding of how self-organized control works. I begin by analyzing the control problem and its solution space. I argue that the two prominent solutions offered by classical cognitive science (centralized control with rich commands, e.g., the Fodorian central systems) and embodied cognitive science (distributed control with simple commands, such as the subsumption architecture by Rodney Brooks) are merely two positions in a two-dimensional solution space. I outline two alternative positions: one is distributed control (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Commentary: Can attention capture visual awareness?Paolo Bartolomeo - 2002 - Psicologica International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology 23 (2):314-317.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neural networks and psychopharmacology.Sbg Park - 1998 - In Dan J. Stein & J. Ludick (eds.), Neural Networks and Psychopathology. Cambridge University Press. pp. 57.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modeling strategies in Stroop with a general architecture of executive control.Tomasz Smoleń & Adam Chuderski - 2010 - In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 931--936.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark