Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Religion, sovereignty, natural rights, and the constituent elements of experience.Jordan B. Peterson - 2006 - Archive for the Psychology of Religion / Archiv für Religionspychologie 28 (1):135-180.
    It is commonly held that the idea of natural rights originated with the ancient Greeks, and was given full form by more modern philosophers such as John Locke, who believed that natural rights were apprehensible primarily to reason. The problem with this broad position is three-fold: first, it is predicated on the presumption that the idea of rights is modern, biologically speaking ; second, it makes it appear that reason and rights are integrally, even causally, linked; finally, it legitimizes debate (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Response time based psychophysics: An added perspective.William M. Petrusic - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):158-159.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Le Physique, le Morphologique, le Symbolique.Jean Petitot - 1990 - Revue de Synthèse 111 (1-2):139-183.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • A functional view of learning.Lewis Petrinovich - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):153-154.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The emperor's old hat.Don Perlis - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):680-681.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Intentionality as internality.Don Perlis & Rosalie Hall - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):151-152.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Exploring the “boundary” between the minds of monkeys and humans.Sidney I. Perloe - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):163-164.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Chaos in brains: Fad or insight?Donald H. Perkel - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):180-181.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A plea for the second functionalist model and the insufficiency of simulation.Josef Perner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):66-67.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A New Deflationary Account of the “Primitive Sense of Selfhood”.Roberto Horácio de Sá Pereira - 2018 - Grazer Philosophische Studien 95 (3):309-328.
    _ Source: _Page Count 20 This paper proposes a new deflationary reading of the metaphor of the “primitive sense of selfhood” in perception and proprioception, usually understood as an “experiential self-reference” that takes place before reflection and any use of concepts. As such, the paper is also a new defense of the old orthodox view that self-consciousness is a highly complex mental phenomenon that requires equally complex concepts. The author’s defense is a clear case of inference to the best explanation. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The nonalgorithmic mind.Roger Penrose - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):692-705.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Representations of movement and representations in movement.Giuseppe Pellizzer & Apostolos P. Georgopoulos - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):216-217.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The archaeology of space: Real and representational.Christopher S. Peebles - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):91-91.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Behavioural artificial intelligence: an agenda for systematic empirical studies of artificial inference.Tore Pedersen & Christian Johansen - 2020 - AI and Society 35 (3):519-532.
    Artificial intelligence receives attention in media as well as in academe and business. In media coverage and reporting, AI is predominantly described in contrasted terms, either as the ultimate solution to all human problems or the ultimate threat to all human existence. In academe, the focus of computer scientists is on developing systems that function, whereas philosophy scholars theorize about the implications of this functionality for human life. In the interface between technology and philosophy there is, however, one imperative aspect (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The impact of the perception of rhythmic music on self-paced oscillatory movements.Mathieu Peckel, Thierry Pozzo & Emmanuel Bigand - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Have Byrne & Hilbert answered Hardin's challenge?Adam Pautz - 2003 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26 (1):44-45.
    I argue that Byrne & Hilbert (B&H) have not answered Hardin's objection to physicalism about color concerning the unitary-binary structure of the colors for two reasons. First, their account of unitary-binary structure seems unsatisfactory. Second, pace B&H, there are no physicalistically acceptable candidates to be the hue-magnitudes. I conclude with a question about the justification of physicalism about color.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • The λ model: Can it walk?Aftab E. Patla - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):775-776.
    Generation of swing phase limb trajectory over obstacles during locomotion should be a reasonable test for the λ model proposed by Feldman and Levin. The observed features such as lack of simple amplitude scaling of endpoint (toe) trajectories for different obstacle heights, complex shaped toe velocity profiles, and exploitation of passive intersegmental dynamics to control limb elevation cannot be adequately explained by the λ model.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modeling Sensory Preference in Speech Motor Planning: A Bayesian Modeling Framework.Jean-François Patri, Julien Diard & Pascal Perrier - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Experimental studies of speech production involving compensations for auditory and somatosensory perturbations and adaptation after training suggest that both types of sensory information are considered to plan and monitor speech production. Interestingly, individual sensory preferences have been observed in this context: subjects who compensate less for somatosensory perturbations compensate more for auditory perturbations, and \textit{vice versa}. We propose to integrate this sensory preference phenomenon in a model of speech motor planning using a probabilistic model in which speech units are characterized (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Architectonic Experience of Body and Space in Augmented Interiors.Isabella Pasqualini, Maria Laura Blefari, Tej Tadi, Andrea Serino & Olaf Blanke - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Quantifying the Degree of Movement Dissimilarity between Two Distinct Action Scenarios: An Exploratory Approach with Procrustes Analysis.Pedro Passos, Tania Campos & Ana Diniz - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Simultaneous processing of features may not be possible.D. M. Parker - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):411-411.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Let us accept a “controlled trade-off” model of motor control.Lloyd D. Partridge - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):773-775.
    The trade-off between force and length of muscle as adjusted by neural signals is a critical fact in the dynamics of motor control. Whether we call it “length-tension effect,” “feedback-like,” “invariant condition,” or “spring-like” is unimportant. We must not let semantics or details of representation obscure the basic physics of effects introduced by this trade-off in muscle.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How was movement controlled before Newton?Lloyd D. Partridge - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):561-561.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Frogs solve Bernstein's problem.Lloyd D. Partridge - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):619-620.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Enhanced Memory for Fair-Related Faces and the Role of Trait Anxiety.Gewnhi Park, Benjamin U. Marsh & Elisha J. Johnson - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    The current research examined whether fair consideration—a social norm that people inherently prefer to confirm—would modulate face recognition. Each neutral face was associated with fair offers or unfair offers via an economic decision task, the Ultimatum Game (UG) task. After the UG, participants were asked to identify the faces of proposers who made different offers. Enhanced memory was observed for fair-related faces compared to unfair-related faces. Furthermore, high trait anxiety was associated with reduced memory for fair-related faces. These results were (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Comment: Journeys to the Center of Emotion.Brian Parkinson - 2013 - Emotion Review 5 (2):180-184.
    Does appraisal co-ordinate emotional responses? Are emotions usually reached via mental representations of relational meaning? This comment considers alternative causal routes in order to assess the centrality of appraisal in the explanation of emotion. Implicit and explicit meaning extraction can certainly help steer the course of emotion-related processes. However, presupposing that appraisals represent the driving force behind all aspects of emotion generation leads to inclusive formulations of appraisal or restrictive formulations of emotion.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • When Neuroscience ‘Touches’ Architecture: From Hapticity to a Supramodal Functioning of the Human Brain.Paolo Papale, Leonardo Chiesi, Alessandra C. Rampinini, Pietro Pietrini & Emiliano Ricciardi - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7:186785.
    In the last decades, the rapid growth of functional brain imaging methodologies allowed cognitive neuroscience to address open questions in philosophy and the social sciences. At the same time, novel insights from cognitive neuroscience research have begun to influence various disciplines, leading to a turn to cognition and emotion in the fields of planning and architectural design. Since 2003, the Academy of Neuroscience for Architecture has been supporting ‘neuro-architecture’ as a way to connect neuroscience and the study of behavioral responses (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Extending Research on Deception in Sport – Combining Perception and Kinematic Approaches.Josefine Panten, Florian Loffing, Joseph Baker & Jörg Schorer - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Architecture and connections of the premotor areas in the rhesus monkey.Deepak N. Pandya & Helen Barbas - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):595-596.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Operationaling “correspondence”.David C. Palmer - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):206-207.
    The research guided by the correspondence metaphor is lauded for its emphasis on functional analysis, but the term “correspondence” itself needs clarification. Of the two terms in the relationship, only one is well defined. It is suggested that behavior at acquisition needs to be analyzed and that molecular principles from the learning laboratory might be useful in doing so.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What spaces? What subjects?Jean Pailhous & Patrick Peruch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):646-647.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The perceptual stability of the visual field: What is calibration for?Jacques Paillard, Michelle Fleury, Normand Teasdale, Chantal Bard & Vincent Nougier - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):272-272.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Jeannerod's representing brain: Image or illusion?Jean Pailhous & Mireille Bonnard - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):215-216.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • When Poetry and Phenomenology Collide.Jeremy Page - 2018 - Journal of Aesthetics and Phenomenology 5 (1):31-51.
    In recent years several scholars have wrestled with the term “poetic thought,” suggesting in various ways there is something distinctive about the nature of meaning as it occurs/unfolds through poetry. In this paper I suggest, in part following the lead of Simon Jarvis, that one of the most fruitful lines of inquiry for exploring this idea lies in a consideration of poetic works through the lens of Heidegger’s early phenomenology. Specifically, I argue that one of the keys to understanding poetic (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Spatial frames for motor control would be commensurate with spatial frames for vision and proprioception, but what about control of energy flows?Christopher C. Pagano & Geoffrey P. Bingham - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):773-773.
    The model identifies a spatial coordinate frame within which the sensorimotor apparatus produces movement. Its spatial nature simplifies its coupling with spatial reference frames used concurrently by vision and proprioception. While the positional reference frame addresses the performance of spatial tasks, it seems to have little to say about movements involving energy expenditure as the principle component of the task.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Taxonomy is older than thinking: Epigenetic decisions.Andrew Packard - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):296-297.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Search Strategies in the Perceptual-Motor Workspace and the Acquisition of Coordination, Control, and Skill.Matheus M. Pacheco, Charley W. Lafe & Karl M. Newell - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Calls as labels: An intriguing theme, but one with limitations.Donald H. Owings - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):162-163.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Seeing where we look: Fixation as extraretinal information.D. Alfred Owens & Edward S. Reed - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):271-272.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Enactivism and the Perception of Others’ Emotions.Søren Overgaard - 2017 - Midwest Studies in Philosophy 41 (1):105-129.
    According to ‘direct social perception’ (DSP) accounts of social cognition, perception may be ‘smart’ enough on its own to inform us about other people’s emotions. Some DSP advocates suggest that ‘smart’ social perception should be conceived along ‘enactive’ lines. In this paper, I suggest that DSP needs social perception to have representational content. This seems in tension with the main versions of enactivism, which deny that perception is representational. I thus present the following challenge to ‘enactive’ versions of DSP: either (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Concepts and how they get that way.Karenleigh A. Overmann - 2019 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 18 (1):153-168.
    Drawing on the material culture of the Ancient Near East as interpreted through Material Engagement Theory, the journey of how material number becomes a conceptual number is traced to address questions of how a particular material form might generate a concept and how concepts might ultimately encompass multiple material forms so that they include but are irreducible to all of them together. Material forms incorporated into the cognitive system affect the content and structure of concepts through their agency and affordances, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Can voluntary movement be understood on the basis of reflex organization?David J. Ostry & Frances E. Wilkinson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):618-619.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Command invariants and the frame of reference for human movement.David J. Ostry, Rafael Laboissière & Paul L. Gribble - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):770-772.
    We describe a solution to the redundancy problem related to that proposed in Feldman & Levin's target article. We suggest that the system may use a fixed mapping between commands organized at the level of degrees of freedom and commands to individual muscles. This proposal eliminates the need to maintain an explicit representation of musculoskeletalgeometry in planning movements.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Brain damage and cognitive dysfunction.Marlene Oscar-Berman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):678-679.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Creative Motor Actions As Emerging from Movement Variability.Dominic Orth, John van der Kamp, Daniel Memmert & Geert J. P. Savelsbergh - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Behavioral Repertoire Influences the Rate and Nature of Learning in Climbing: Implications for Individualized Learning Design in Preparation for Extreme Sports Participation.Dominic Orth, Keith Davids, Jia-Yi Chow, Eric Brymer & Ludovic Seifert - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The world as an outside iconic memory – no strong internal metric means no problem of visual stability.J. Kevin O'Regan - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):270-271.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What do double dissociations prove?Guy C. Orden, Bruce F. Pennington & Gregory O. Stone - 2001 - Cognitive Science 25 (1):111-172.
    Brain damage may doubly dissociate cognitive modules, but the practice of revealing dissociations is predicated on modularity being true (T. Shallice, 1988). This article questions the utility of assuming modularity, as it examines a paradigmatic double dissociation of reading modules. Reading modules illustrate two general problems. First, modularity fails to converge on a fixed set of exclusionary criteria that define pure cases. As a consequence, competing modular theories force perennial quests for purer cases, which simply perpetuates growth in the list (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • An enactive account of placebo effects.Giulio Ongaro & Dave Ward - 2017 - Biology and Philosophy 32 (4):507-533.
    Placebos are commonly defined as ineffective treatments. They are treatments that lack a known mechanism linking their properties to the properties of the condition on which treatment aims to intervene. Given this, the fact that placebos can have substantial therapeutic effects looks puzzling. The puzzle, we argue, arises from the relationship placebos present between culturally meaningful entities, our intentional relationship to the environment and bodily effects. How can a mere attitude toward a treatment result in appropriate bodily changes? We argue (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • What Ekman really said.Mats Olsson, Kathleen Harder & John C. Baird - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):157-158.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark