Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The dark and bright side of the numbers: how emotions influence mental number line accuracy and bias.Saied Sabaghypour, Farhad Farkhondeh Tale Navi, Elena Kulkova, Parnian Abaduz, Negin Zirak & Mohammad Ali Nazari - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    The traditional view of cognition as detached from emotions is recently being questioned. This study aimed to investigate the influence of emotional valence on the accuracy and bias in the representation of numbers on the mental number line (MNL). The study included 164 participants who were randomly assigned into two groups with induced positive and negative emotional valence using matched arousal film clips. Participants performed a computerised number-to-position (CNP) task to estimate the position of numbers on a horizontal line. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sadness facilitates “deeper” reading comprehension: a behavioural and eye tracking study.Caitlin Mills, Rosy Southwell & Sidney K. D’Mello - 2024 - Cognition and Emotion 38 (1):171-179.
    Reading is one of the most common everyday activities, yet research elucidating how affective influence reading processes and outcomes is sparse with inconsistent results. To investigate this question, we randomly assigned participants (N = 136) to happiness (positive affect), sadness (negative affect), and neutral video-induction conditions prior to engaging in self-paced reading of a long, complex science text. Participants completed assessments targeting multiple levels of comprehension (e.g. recognising factual information, integrating different textual components, and open-ended responses of concepts from memory) (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Social robots as depictions of social agents.Herbert H. Clark & Kerstin Fischer - 2023 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46:e21.
    Social robots serve people as tutors, caretakers, receptionists, companions, and other social agents. People know that the robots are mechanical artifacts, yet they interact with them as if they were actual agents. How is this possible? The proposal here is that people construe social robots not as social agents per se, but as depictions of social agents. They interpret them much as they interpret ventriloquist dummies, hand puppets, virtual assistants, and other interactive depictions of people and animals. Depictions as a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Emotional Differences in Young and Older Adults: Films as Mood Induction Procedure.Luz Fernández-Aguilar, Jorge Ricarte, Laura Ros & Jose M. Latorre - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:372611.
    Film clips are proven to be one of the most efficient techniques in emotional induction. However, there is scant literature on the effect of this procedure in older adults and, specifically, the effect of using different positive stimuli. Thus, the aim of the present study was to examine emotional differences between young and older adults and to know how a set of film clips works as mood induction procedure in older adults, especially, when trying to elicit attachment-related emotions. To this (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A New Standardized Emotional Film Database for Asian Culture.Yaling Deng, Meng Yang & Renlai Zhou - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • A Review of Dynamic Datasets for Facial Expression Research. [REVIEW]Eva G. Krumhuber, Lina Skora, Dennis Küster & Linyun Fou - 2017 - Emotion Review 9 (3):280-292.
    Temporal dynamics have been increasingly recognized as an important component of facial expressions. With the need for appropriate stimuli in research and application, a range of databases of dynamic facial stimuli has been developed. The present article reviews the existing corpora and describes the key dimensions and properties of the available sets. This includes a discussion of conceptual features in terms of thematic issues in dataset construction as well as practical features which are of applied interest to stimulus usage. To (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Networks of self-defining memories as a contributing factor to emotional openness.Iliane Houle, Frederick L. Philippe, Serge Lecours & Josiane Roulez - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (2):363-370.
    Emotional openness is characterised by a capacity to tolerate threatening self-relevant material and an interest towards new emotional situations. We investigated how specific networks of memories could be an important contributing factor to emotional openness. At Phase 1, participants completed measures of personality traits and emotional intelligence, described a self-defining memory, provided other memories associated with it, and rated the valence of each of their memories. A score assessing the complexity of this memory network, comprising the number of memories reported (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Internally and externally generated emotions in people with acquired brain injury: preservation of emotional experience after right hemisphere lesions.Christian E. Salas Riquelme, Darinka Radovic, Osvaldo Castro & Oliver H. Turnbull - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:121484.
    The study of emotional changes after brain injury has contributed enormously to the understanding of the neural basis of emotion. However, little attention has been placed on the methods used to elicit emotional responses in people with brain damage. Of particular interest are subjects with right hemisphere [RH] cortical lesions, who have been described as presenting impairment in emotional processing. In this article, an internal and external mood induction procedure [MIP] was used to trigger positive and negative emotions, in a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The effects of culture and context on perceptions of robotic facial expressions.Casey C. Bennett & Selma Šabanović - 2015 - Interaction Studies 16 (2):272-302.
    We report two experimental studies of human perceptions of robotic facial expressions while systematically varying context effects and the cultural background of subjects (n = 93). Except for Fear, East Asian and Western subjects were not significantly different in recognition rates, and, while Westerners were better at judging affect from mouth movement alone, East Asians were not any better at judging affect based on eye/brow movement alone. Moreover, context effects appeared capable of over-riding such cultural differences, most notably for Fear. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Automatic control of negative emotions: Evidence that structured practice increases the efficiency of emotion regulation.Spyros Christou-Champi, Tom F. D. Farrow & Thomas L. Webb - 2015 - Cognition and Emotion 29 (2):319-331.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Brief report.Johannes Hewig, Dirk Hagemann, Jan Seifert, Mario Gollwitzer, Ewald Naumann & Dieter Bartussek - 2005 - Cognition and Emotion 19 (7):1095-1109.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Epinephrine, Arousal, and Emotion: A New Look at Two-factor Theory.Elizabeth S. Mezzacappa - 1999 - Cognition and Emotion 13 (2):181-199.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Emotion Regulation: Past, Present, Future.James J. Gross - 1999 - Cognition and Emotion 13 (5):551-573.
    Modern emotion theories emphasise the adaptive value of emotions. Emotions are by no means always helpful, however. They often must be regulated. The study of emotion regulation has its origins in the psychoanalytic and stress and coping traditions. Recently, increased interest in emotion regulation has led to crucial boundary ambiguities that now threaten progress in this domain. It is argued that distinctions need to be made between (1) regulation of emotion and regulation by emotion; (2) emotion regulation in self and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   73 citations  
  • Mood-specific effects on appraisal and emotion judgements.Matthias Siemer - 2001 - Cognition and Emotion 15 (4):453-485.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Heartbeat detection and the experience of emotions.Stefan Wiens, Elizabeth S. Mezzacappa & Edward S. Katkin - 2000 - Cognition and Emotion 14 (3):417-427.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • Do negative mood states impact moral reasoning?Brian Barger & W. Pitt Derryberry - 2013 - Journal of Moral Education 42 (4):443-459.
    This paper presents three studies exploring the relationship between emotional responses to classic cognitive developmental moral dilemmas and moral reasoning indices as measured by the Defining Issues Test (DIT). Each study indicated that certain moral dilemmas elicit varying levels of anger and sadness as compared to a neutral baseline. In each study, decreased moral reasoning was observed in those instances where reports in both sadness and anger were high following a dilemma. This did not occur, however, in those instances where (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Virtually real emotions and the paradox of fiction: Implications for the use of virtual environments in psychological research.Garry Young - 2010 - Philosophical Psychology 23 (1):1-21.
    Many of the psychological studies carried out within virtual environments are motivated by the idea that virtual research findings are generalizable to the non-virtual world. This idea is vulnerable to the paradox of fiction, which questions whether it is possible to express genuine emotion toward a character (or event) known to be fictitious. As many of these virtual studies are designed to elicit, broadly speaking, emotional responses through interactions with fictional characters (avatars) or objects/places, the issue raised by the paradox (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Because you had a bad day: the role of negative affect and justification in self-control failure.Ally M. Heiland & Jennifer C. Veilleux - 2022 - Cognition and Emotion 36 (5):912-927.
    Justification thinking (using excuses to “allow” giving into temptation) has been identified as a potential link between negative affect and self-control failure. We hypothesised that negative affect would prompt greater justification thinking, specifically deservingness thinking (i.e. “I deserve a treat”), and tested this for both inhibitory (temptation is to approach reward; self-control is to inhibit) and initiatory (temptation is to refrain from action, self-control is to initiate action) hypothetical self-control dilemmas. We found that only for inhibitory self-control (Study 1; N (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The effects of culture and context on perceptions of robotic facial expressions.Casey C. Bennett & Selma Šabanović - 2015 - Interaction Studies 16 (2):272-302.
    We report two experimental studies of human perceptions of robotic facial expressions while systematically varying context effects and the cultural background of subjects (n = 93). Except for Fear, East Asian and Western subjects were not significantly different in recognition rates, and, while Westerners were better at judging affect from mouth movement alone, East Asians were not any better at judging affect based on eye/brow movement alone. Moreover, context effects appeared capable of over-riding such cultural differences, most notably for Fear. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Psychophysiological Effects of Downregulating Negative Emotions: Insights From a Meta-Analysis of Healthy Adults.Jenny Zaehringer, Christine Jennen-Steinmetz, Christian Schmahl, Gabriele Ende & Christian Paret - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Arousal Rules: An Empirical Investigation into the Aesthetic Experience of Cross-Modal Perception with Emotional Visual Music.Irene Eunyoung Lee, Charles-Francois V. Latchoumane & Jaeseung Jeong - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Emotional State of Being Moved Elicited by Films: A Comparison With Several Positive Emotions.Kenta Kimura, Satoshi Haramizu, Kazue Sanada & Akiko Oshida - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Development and Validation of Verbal Emotion Vignettes in Portuguese, English, and German.Tanja S. H. Wingenbach, Leticia Y. Morello, Ana L. Hack & Paulo S. Boggio - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Investigating the Effects of Anger and Guilt on Unethical Behavior: A Dual-Process Approach.Daphna Motro, Lisa D. Ordóñez, Andrea Pittarello & David T. Welsh - 2018 - Journal of Business Ethics 152 (1):133-148.
    Although emotion has become one of the most popular research areas within organizational scholarship, few studies have considered its connection with unethical behavior. Using dual-process theory, we expand on the rationalist perspective within the field of behavioral ethics by considering the process through which two discrete emotions, anger and guilt, influence unethical behavior. Across two studies using different methodologies, we found that anger increases unethical behavior whereas guilt reduces unethical behavior. These effects were mediated by impulsive and deliberative processing. Overall, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Cold Side-Effect Effect: Affect Does Not Mediate the Influence of Moral Considerations in Intentionality Judgments.Rodrigo Díaz - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8:295.
    Research has consistently shown that people consider harmful side effects of an action more intentional than helpful side effects. This phenomenon is known as the side- effect effect (SEE), which refers to the influence of moral considerations in judgments of intentionality and other non-moral concepts. There is an ongoing debate about how to explain this asymmetric pattern of judgment and the psychological factors involved in it. It has been posited that affective reactions to agents that bring about harmful side- effects (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Experimental Methods for Inducing Basic Emotions: A Qualitative Review.Ewa Siedlecka & Thomas F. Denson - 2019 - Emotion Review 11 (1):87-97.
    Experimental emotion inductions provide the strongest causal evidence of the effects of emotions on psychological and physiological outcomes. In the present qualitative review, we evaluated five common experimental emotion induction techniques: visual stimuli, music, autobiographical recall, situational procedures, and imagery. For each technique, we discuss the extent to which they induce six basic emotions: anger, disgust, surprise, happiness, fear, and sadness. For each emotion, we discuss the relative influences of the induction methods on subjective emotional experience and physiological responses. Based (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Non-response to sad mood induction: implications for emotion research.Jonathan Rottenberg, Maria Kovacs & Ilya Yaroslavsky - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (3):431-436.
    Experimental induction of sad mood states is a mainstay of laboratory research on affect and cognition, mood regulation, and mood disorders. Typically, the success of such mood manipulations is reported as a statistically significant pre- to post-induction change in the self-rated intensity of the target affect. The present commentary was motivated by an unexpected finding in one of our studies concerning the response rate to a well-validated sad mood induction. Using the customary statistical approach, we found a significant mean increase (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test: Validity and Relationship with Cardiovascular Stress-Responses.Melanie M. van der Ploeg, Jos F. Brosschot, Julian F. Thayer & Bart Verkuil - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Hypnosis, hypnotic suggestibility, memory, and involvement in films.Reed Maxwell, Steven Jay Lynn & Liam Condon - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 33:170-184.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Fear shapes information acquisition in decisions from experience.Renato Frey, Ralph Hertwig & Jörg Rieskamp - 2014 - Cognition 132 (1):90-99.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • One night of sleep is insufficient to achieve sleep-to-forget emotional decontextualisation processes.Gaétane Deliens & Philippe Peigneux - 2014 - Cognition and Emotion 28 (4):698-706.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Do (un)certainty appraisal tendencies reverse the influence of emotions on risk taking in sequential tasks?Virginie Bagneux, Thierry Bollon & Cécile Dantzer - 2012 - Cognition and Emotion 26 (3):568-576.
    According to the Appraisal-Tendency Framework (Han, Lerner, & Keltner, 2007), certainty-associated emotions increase risk taking compared with uncertainty-associated emotions. To date, this general effect has only been shown in static judgement and decision-making paradigms; therefore, the present study tested the effect of certainty on risk taking in a sequential decision-making task. We hypothesised that the effect would be reversed due to the kind of processing involved, as certainty is considered to encourage heuristic processing that takes into account the emotional cues (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Anxiety and disgust: Evidence for a unidirectional relationship.Sarah Marzillier & Graham Davey - 2005 - Cognition and Emotion 19 (5):729-750.
    This paper reports the results of three studies using mood induction procedures (MIPs) designed to investigate the relationship between anxiety and disgust. Study 1 used guided imagery vignettes (i.e., asking participants to imagine themselves in a series of described situations) and music (Mayer, Allen, & Beauregard, 1995). Study 2 used video clips (Gross & Levenson, 1995). Study 3 used autobiographical recall and music (Blagden & Craske, 1996). In order to be as sure as possible that target moods were being induced, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Emotion and Regulation are One!Arvid Kappas - 2011 - Emotion Review 3 (1):17-25.
    Emotions are foremost self-regulating processes that permit rapid responses and adaptations to situations of personal concern. They have biological bases and are shaped ontogenetically via learning and experience. Many situations and events of personal concern are social in nature. Thus, social exchanges play an important role in learning about rules and norms that shape regulation processes. I argue that (a) emotions often are actively auto-regulating—the behavior implied by the emotional reaction bias to the eliciting event or situation modifies or terminates (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Do You See What I See? Effectiveness of 360-Degree vs. 2D Video Ads Using a Neuroscience Approach.Jose M. Ausin-Azofra, Enrique Bigne, Carla Ruiz, Javier Marín-Morales, Jaime Guixeres & Mariano Alcañiz - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12:612717.
    This study compares cognitive and emotional responses to 360-degree vs. static (2D) videos in terms of visual attention, brand recognition, engagement of the prefrontal cortex, and emotions. Hypotheses are proposed based on the interactivity literature, cognitive overload, advertising response model and motivation, opportunity, and ability theoretical frameworks, and tested using neurophysiological tools: electroencephalography, eye-tracking, electrodermal activity, and facial coding. The results revealed that gaze view depends on ad content, visual attention paid being lower in 360-degree FMCG ads than in 2D (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Facial Expressions of Basic Emotions in Japanese Laypeople.Wataru Sato, Sylwia Hyniewska, Kazusa Minemoto & Sakiko Yoshikawa - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Emotion Elicitation: A Comparison of Pictures and Films.Meike K. Uhrig, Nadine Trautmann, Ulf Baumgärtner, Rolf-Detlef Treede, Florian Henrich, Wolfgang Hiller & Susanne Marschall - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • I’m so angry I could help you: Moral outrage as a driver of victim compensation.Erik W. Thulin & Cristina Bicchieri - 2016 - Social Philosophy and Policy 32 (2):146-160.
    :Recent behavioral economics studies have shown that third parties compensate players in Dictator, Ultimatum, and Trust games. However, there are almost no studies about what drives third parties to compensate victims in such games. It can be argued that compensation is a form of helping; and helping behavior, in a variety of forms, has been widely researched, especially with regard to motivators. Previous work on helping behavior has focused on empathic concern as a primary driver. In sharp contrast, anger is (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Positive Emotions Speed Recovery from the Cardiovascular Sequelae of Negative Emotions.Barbara L. Fredrickson & Robert W. Levenson - 1998 - Cognition and Emotion 12 (2):191-220.
    Two studies tested the hypothesis that certain positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. In Study 1, 60 subjects (Ss) viewed an initial fear-eliciting film, and were randomly assigned to view a secondary film that elicited: (a) contentment; (b) amusement; (c) neutrality; or (d) sadness. Compared to Ss who viewed the neutral and sad secondary films, those who viewed the positive films exhibited more rapid returns to pre-film levels of cardiovascular activation. In Study 2, 72 Ss (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • That's disgusting!…, but very amusing: Mixed feelings of amusement and disgust.Scott H. Hemenover & Ulrich Schimmack - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (5):1102-1113.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Emotion control values and responding to an anger provocation in Asian-American and European-American individuals.Iris B. Mauss, Emily A. Butler, Nicole A. Roberts & Ann Chu - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (6):1026-1043.
    The present research examined whether Asian-American (AA) versus European-American (EA) women differed in experiential, expressive, or autonomic physiological responding to a laboratory anger provocation and assessed the mediating role of values about emotional control. Results indicate that AA participants reported and behaviourally displayed less anger than EA participants, while there were no group differences in physiological responses. Observed differences in emotional responses were partially mediated by emotion control values, suggesting a potential mechanism for effects of cultural background on anger responding.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Effects of Sleep Fragmentation and Induced Mood on Pain Tolerance and Pain Sensitivity in Young Healthy Adults.Ragna Rosseland, Ståle Pallesen, Inger Hilde Nordhus, Dagfinn Matre & Tone Blågestad - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:356779.
    _Background:_ Experimental research exploring the sleep/pain-relationship has typically focused on total or partial sleep deprivation, hereby failing to reproduce the mere fragmented sleep pattern typically observed in patients with chronic pain. Further, little research is done on how affect moderates the sleep–pain relationship after sleep fragmentation. The present study sought to clarify the relationship between pain, sleep and positive and negative affect. _Methods:_ In an experimental counterbalanced crossover design, 35 healthy young adults were subjected to several pain measures after one (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Presenting KAPODI – The Searchable Database of Emotional Stimuli Sets.Kathrin Diconne, Georgios K. Kountouriotis, Aspasia E. Paltoglou, Andrew Parker & Thomas J. Hostler - 2022 - Emotion Review 14 (1):84-95.
    Emotion Review, Volume 14, Issue 1, Page 84-95, January 2022. Emotional stimuli such as images, words, or video clips are often used in studies researching emotion. New sets are continuously being published, creating an immense number of available sets and complicating the task for researchers who are looking for suitable stimuli. This paper presents the KAPODI-database of emotional stimuli sets that are freely available or available upon request. Over 45 aspects including over 25 key set characteristics have been extracted and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Audio Described vs. Audiovisual Porn: Cortisol, Heart Rate and Engagement in Visually Impaired vs. Sighted Participants.Ana M. Rojo López, Marina Ramos Caro & Laura Espín López - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Audio description remains the cornerstone of accessibility for visually impaired audiences to all sorts of audiovisual content, including porn. Existing work points to the efficacy of audio description to guarantee immersion and emotional engagement, but evidence on its role in sexual arousal and engagement in porn is still scant. The present study takes on this challenge by comparing sighted and visually impaired participants’ experiences with porn in terms of their physiological response [i.e., cortisol and heart rate ] and self-report measures (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • No Association Between Autonomic Functioning and Psychopathy and Aggression in Multi-Problem Young Adults.Josjan Zijlmans, Reshmi Marhe, Laura van Duin, Marie-Jolette A. Luijks, Floor Bevaart & Arne Popma - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    BackgroundAberrant functioning of the autonomic nervous system is an important factor in the occurrence of antisocial behavior. Baseline autonomic functioning and the responsivity of the ANS have been related to psychopathic traits and aggression. Here we investigated whether a naturalistic sample of male multi-problem young adults present with similar autonomic deficits in relation to their psychopathy and aggression as previous studies observed in clinical samples.MethodsIn a sample of 112 multi-problem young adults, baseline autonomic functioning and autonomic responsivity to emotional stimuli (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Disgust selectively dampens value-independent risk-taking for potential gains.Yu Tong, Jingwei Sun, Nicholas D. Wright & Jian Li - 2020 - Cognition 200 (C):104266.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rethinking the Principles of Emotion Taxonomy.Assaf Kron - 2019 - Emotion Review 11 (3):226-233.
    This article examines whether a functionalist approach to emotion classification is a research program that can feasibly be implemented in an experimental environment. I suggest that this is a promise perhaps impossible to keep. The crux of the argument is that if functional taxonomy is to go the full distance and shape experimental conditions to the new boundaries, then stimuli/experimental manipulations must be selected based on functional principles. But this seems implausible or even impossible. I conclude that emotion taxonomy, and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A standardised database of Chinese emotional film clips.Yan Ge, Guozhen Zhao, Yulin Zhang, Rebecca J. Houston & Jinjing Song - 2018 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (5):976-990.
    ABSTRACTFilm clips are widely used in emotion research due to their relatively high ecological validity. Although researchers have established various film clip sets for different cultures, the few that exist related to Chinese culture do not adequately address positive emotions. The main purposes of the present study were to establish a standardised database of Chinese emotional film clips that could elicit more categories of reported positive emotions compared to the existing databases and to expand the available film clips that can (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Many moral buttons or just one? Evidence from emotional facial expressions.Laura Franchin, Janet Geipel, Constantinos Hadjichristidis & Luca Surian - 2019 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (5):943-958.
    ABSTRACTWe investigated whether moral violations involving harm selectively elicit anger, whereas purity violations selectively elicit disgust, as predicted by the Moral Foundations Theory. We analysed participants’ spontaneous facial expressions as they listened to scenarios depicting moral violations of harm and purity. As predicted by MFT, anger reactions were elicited more frequently by harmful than by impure actions. However, violations of purity elicited more smiling reactions and expressions of anger than of disgust. This effect was found both in a classic set (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Three decades of Cognition & Emotion: A brief review of past highlights and future prospects.Klaus Rothermund & Sander L. Koole - 2018 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (1):1-12.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations