- Is human information processing conscious?Max Velmans - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):651-69.details
|
|
Evidence against epiphenomenalism.Ned Block - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):670-672.details
|
|
Consciousness from a first-person perspective.Max Velmans - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):702-726.details
|
|
The Structure of Scientific Theories.Rasmus Grønfeldt Winther - 2015 - Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.details
|
|
From knowledge to wisdom: a revolution in the aims and methods of science.Nicholas Maxwell - 1984 - Oxford: Blackwell.details
|
|
The function of consciousness or of information?David Navon - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):690-691.details
|
|
Scientific change: Philosophical models and historical research.Larry Laudan, Arthur Donovan, Rachel Laudan, Peter Barker, Harold Brown, Jarrett Leplin, Paul Thagard & Steve Wykstra - 1986 - Synthese 69 (2):141 - 223.details
|
|
Invariance as a basis for necessity and laws.Gila Sher - 2021 - Philosophical Studies 178 (12):3945-3974.details
|
|
Consciousness may still have a processing role to play.Robert Van Gulick - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):699-700.details
|
|
Axiomatic rationality and ecological rationality.Gerd Gigerenzer - 2019 - Synthese 198 (4):3547-3564.details
|
|
Darwin and the political economists: Divergence of character.Silvan S. Schweber - 1980 - Journal of the History of Biology 13 (2):195-289.details
|
|
Realism, perspectivism, and disagreement in science.Michela Massimi - 2019 - Synthese 198 (Suppl 25):6115-6141.details
|
|
International Handbook of Research in History, Philosophy and Science Teaching.Michael R. Matthews (ed.) - 2014 - Springer.details
|
|
Understanding awareness at the neuronal level.Christof Koch & Francis Crick - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):683-685.details
|
|
A sharper image: the quest of science and recursive production of objective realities.Julio Michael Stern - 2020 - Principia: An International Journal of Epistemology 24 (2):255-297.details
|
|
Teaching the Philosophical and Worldview Components of Science.Michael R. Matthews - 2009 - Science & Education 18 (6-7):697-728.details
|
|
Science, Worldviews and Education.Michael R. Matthews - 2014 - In International Handbook of Research in History, Philosophy and Science Teaching. Springer. pp. 1585-1635.details
|
|
Truth and beauty in scientific reason.James W. Mcallister - 1989 - Synthese 78 (1):25 - 51.details
|
|
Induction and scientific realism: Einstein versus Van Fraassen part three: Einstein, aim-oriented empiricism and the discovery of special and general relativity.Nicholas Maxwell - 1993 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 44 (2):275-305.details
|
|
A coherentist conception of ad hoc hypotheses.Samuel Schindler - 2018 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 67:54-64.details
|
|
On Kepler's awareness of the problem of experimental error.Giora Hon - 1987 - Annals of Science 44 (6):545-591.details
|
|
Simulation Methods for an Abductive System in Science.Tom Addis, Jan Townsend Addis, Dave Billinge, David Gooding & Bart-Floris Visscher - 2008 - Foundations of Science 13 (1):37-52.details
|
|
Trumping Naturalism Revisited.A. C. Paseau - 2024 - In Sophia Arbeiter & Juliette Kennedy, The Philosophy of Penelope Maddy. Springer. pp. 267-290.details
|
|
Analogy and creativity in the works of Johannes Kepler.Dedre Gentner, Sarah Brem, Ron Ferguson, Philip Wolff, Arthur B. Markman & Ken Forbus - 1997 - In T. B. Ward, S. M. Smith & J. Vaid, Creative Thought: An Investigation of Conceptual Structures and Processes. American Psychological Association.details
|
|
Developing concepts of consciousness.Aaron Sloman - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):694-695.details
|
|
Radical views on cognition and the dynamics of scientific change.Pierre Steiner - 2019 - Synthese 198 (Suppl 1):547-569.details
|
|
Conscious functions and brain processes.Benjamin Libet - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):685-686.details
|
|
A lawful first-person psychology involving a causal consciousness: A psychoanalytic solution.Howard Shevrin - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):693-694.details
|
|
A curious coincidence? Consciousness as an object of scientific scrutiny fits our personal experience remarkably well.Bernard J. Baars - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):669-670.details
|
|
The processing of information is not conscious, but its products often are.George Mandler - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):688-689.details
|
|
Moral Imagination, Trading Zones, and the Role of the Ethicist in Nanotechnology.Michael E. Gorman, Patricia H. Werhane & Nathan Swami - 2009 - NanoEthics 3 (3):185-195.details
|
|
Biology and the social sciences.Edward O. Wilson - 1990 - Zygon 25 (3):245-262.details
|
|
Research Integrity and Everyday Practice of Science.Frederick Grinnell - 2013 - Science and Engineering Ethics 19 (3):685-701.details
|
|
Tendiendo Puentes: Los Themata de Holton y su Nexo con Marcos Filosóficos Establecidos en la Investigación Científica.Georges Alahou - 2024 - Culturas Cientificas 5 (1):03-19.details
|
|
The role of themata in science.Gerald Holton - 1996 - Foundations of Physics 26 (4):453-465.details
|
|
(1 other version)Ernst Mach.Paul Pojman - 2008 - Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.details
|
|
A New Mechanism for Transfer Between Conceptual Domains in Scientific Discovery and Education.Gérard Collet, Andrée Tiberghien & Antoine Cornuéjols - 2000 - Foundations of Science 5 (2):129-155.details
|
|
The ombudsman for research practice.Ruth L. Fischbach & Diane C. Gilbert - 1995 - Science and Engineering Ethics 1 (4):389-402.details
|
|
Notes on the cultural significance of the sciences.Wallis A. Suchting - 1994 - Science & Education 3 (1):1-56.details
|
|
How Einstein Made Asymmetry Disappear: Symmetry and Relativity in 1905.Bernard R. Goldstein & Giora Hon - 2005 - Archive for History of Exact Sciences 59 (5):437-544.details
|
|
The ombudsman for research practice.Dr Ruth L. Fischbach & Diane C. Gilbert - 1995 - Science and Engineering Ethics 1 (4):389-402.details
|
|
Reasons for doubting the existence of even epiphenomenal consciousness.Georges Rey - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):691-692.details
|
|
Conceptual change, cross-theoretical explanation, and the unity of science.Richard M. Burian - 1975 - Synthese 32 (1-2):1 - 28.details
|
|
The substantivalist view of spacetime proposed by Minkowski and its educational implications.Olivia Levrini - 2002 - Science & Education 11 (6):601-617.details
|
|
(2 other versions)Simplicity and observability: When are particles elementary?Kostas Gavroglu - 1989 - Synthese 79 (3):89 - 100.details
|
|
Six things Popper would like biologists not to ignore: In memoriam, Karl Raimund Popper, 1902–1994.Tom Settle - 1996 - Biology and Philosophy 11 (2):141-159.details
|
|
Damn! There goes that ghost again!Keith E. Stanovich - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):696-698.details
|
|
Linking Science to Common Sense.Jorge Correia Jesuino - 2008 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 38 (4):393-409.details
|
|
Styles of Reasoning in Early to mid-Victorian Life Research: Analysis:Synthesis and Palaetiology.James Elwick - 2007 - Journal of the History of Biology 40 (1):35-69.details
|
|
``Why study history for science?''.Jane Maienschein - 2000 - Biology and Philosophy 15 (3):339-348.details
|
|