Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Concepts of drift and selection in “the great snail debate” of the 1950s and early 1960s.Roberta L. Millstein - 2009 - In Joe Cain Michael Ruse (ed.), Descended from Darwin: Insights into the History of Evolutionary Studies, 1900-1970. American Philosophical Society.
    Recently, much philosophical discussion has centered on the best way to characterize the concepts of random drift and natural selection, and, in particular, whether selection and drift can be conceptually distinguished (Beatty, 1984; Brandon, 2005; Hodge, 1983, 1987; Millstein, 2002, 2005; Pfeifer, 2005; Shanahan, 1992; Stephens, 2004). These authors all contend, to a greater or lesser degree, that their concepts make sense of biological practice. So it should be instructive to see how the concepts of drift and selection were distinguished (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • From the reaktionsNorm to the adaptive Norm: The Norm of reaction, 1909–1960. [REVIEW]Sahotra Sarkar - 1999 - Biology and Philosophy 14 (2):235-252.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Organic selection: Proximate environmental effects on the evolution of morphology and behaviour. [REVIEW]Brian K. Hall - 2001 - Biology and Philosophy 16 (2):215-237.
    Organic selection (the Baldwin Effect) by which an environmentally elicitedphenotypic adaptation comes under genotypic control following selectionwas proposed independently in 1896 by the psychologists James Baldwinand Conwy Lloyd Morgan and by the paleontologist Henry Fairfield Osborn.Modified forms of organic selection were proposed as autonomization bySchmalhausen in 1938, as genetic assimilation by Waddington in 1942, andas an explanation for evolution in changing environments or for speciationby Matsuda and West-Eberhard in the 1980s. Organic selection as amechanism mediating proximate environmental effects on the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Harmony from discord.Raphal Falk & Sahotra Sarkar - 1992 - Biology and Philosophy 7 (4):463-472.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Organizing Microbial Diversity and Interspecies Relations through Diagrams: Trees, Maps, and the Visual Semiotics of the Living.Valeria Burgio & Roberta Raffaetà - forthcoming - Biosemiotics:1-28.
    This paper aims to understand how and why tree diagrams are of central importance to microbiome scientists in their practices of meaning making. The interfaces that scientists use are, in fact, topological structures that organize the genetic data generated by sequencing technology. They establish relationships among microbes and also between microbes and the conditions of the ecological niche they help construct. The tree structure is a powerful _topos_ of knowledge organization in Western culture. However, biomolecular research has revealed the existence (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The T‐locus – inspiration and distraction?Robert P. Erickson - 2024 - Bioessays 46 (12):2400021.
    The T/t locus was a major focus of study by mouse geneticists during the 20th century. In the 70s, as the study of cell surface antigens controlling transplantation antigens was taking off, several laboratories hypothesized that alleles of this locus would control cell surface antigens important for embryonic development. One such antigen, the embryonal carcinoma F9 antigen was said to be an example. Other antigens were described on sperm and embryos that were said to be controlled by alleles at the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Booknotes.R. M. - 1994 - Biology and Philosophy 9 (4):507-514.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Booknotes.R. M. - 1994 - Biology and Philosophy 9 (2):253-259.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Teleology and theology. On the specificity of teleological explanations.Gabriele De Anna - 2018 - European Journal for Philosophy of Religion 10 (3):27-50.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The only wrong cell is the dead one: On the enactive approach to normativity.Manuel Heras-Escribano, Jason Noble & Manuel De Pinedo García - 2013 - In Heras-Escribano Manuel, Noble Jason & Pinedo García Manuel De (eds.), Pietro Liò et al. (eds.) Advances in Artificial Life (ECAL 2013). pp. 665-670.
    In this paper we challenge the notion of ‘normativity’ used by some enactive approaches to cognition. We define some varieties of enactivism and their assumptions and make explicit the reasoning behind the co-emergence of individuality and normativity. Then we argue that appealing to dispositions for explaining some living processes can be more illuminating than claiming that all such processes are normative. For this purpose, we will present some considerations, inspired by Wittgenstein, regarding norm-establishing and norm-following and show that attributions of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Unifying biology: The evolutionary synthesis and evolutionary biology.V. B. Smocovitis - 1992 - Journal of the History of Biology 25 (1):1-65.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   111 citations  
  • Pierre Teilhard de Chardin the founder of a new pseudo-christian evolutionary mysticism.J. J. Duyvené de Wit - 1964 - Philosophia Reformata 29:114.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Punctuated equilibria and phyletic gradualism: Even partners can be good friends.J. C. Von Vaupel Klein - 1994 - Acta Biotheoretica 42 (1):15-48.
    The allegedly alternative theories of Phyletic Gradualism and Punctuated Equilibria are examined as regards the nature of their differences. The explanatory value of both models is determined by establishing their actual connection with reality. It is concluded that they are to be considered complementary rather than mutually exclusive at all levels of infraspecific, specific, and supraspecific evolution. So, in order to be described comprehensively, the pathways of evolution require at least two distinct models, each based on a discrete range of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Phyletic Gradualism versus Punctuated Equilibria: Why case histories do not suffice.J. C. Von Vaupel Klein - 1995 - Acta Biotheoretica 43 (3):259-278.
    Many attempts have been made at supporting either one of the allegedly complementary divergence models Phyletic Gradualism and Punctuated Equilibria by patterns found in specific fossil sequences. However, assessing each model's connection with reality via such “individual case histories” appears not to constitute a relevant approach. Instead, in order to correctly establish the possible merits of both concepts, the claims of each have to be verified against general evolutionary theory. This is being pointed out herein by analyzing cladogenesis at the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Anthropological criticisms of Dunbar's theory of the origin of language.Robert Bates Graber - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):705-705.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Confounded correlations, again.Terrence W. Deacon - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):698-699.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Do grooming and speech really serve homologous functions?Merlin Donald - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):700-701.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Brain expansion: Thoughts on hunting or reckoning kinship – or both?C. Loring Brace - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):695-696.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Communicative acts and drug-induced feelings.Irene M. Pepperberg - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):659-660.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Communication versus discrimination.Valerie Gray Hardcastle - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):649-650.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Some consequences of selection.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):502-510.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Natural selection and operant behavior.Wanda Wyrwicka - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):501-502.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Fitting culture into a Skinner box.C. R. Hallpike - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):489-490.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Fitness, reinforcement, underlying mechanisms.Alexander Rosenberg - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):495-496.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the status of causal modes.Robert C. Bolles - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):482-483.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The wider context of selection by consequences.Thomas J. Gamble - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):488-489.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Selection by consequences.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):477-481.
    Human behavior is the joint product of (i) contingencies of survival responsible for natural selection, and (ii) contingencies of reinforcement responsible for the repertoires of individuals, including (iii) the special contingencies maintained by an evolved social environment. Selection by consequences is a causal mode found only in living things, or in machines made by living things. It was first recognized in natural selection: Reproduction, a first consequence, led to the evolution of cells, organs, and organisms reproducing themselves under increasingly diverse (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   69 citations  
  • Primate group size, brains and communication: A New World perspective.Charles H. Janson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):711-712.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans.R. I. M. Dunbar - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):681-694.
    Group size is a function of relative neocortical volume in nonhuman primates. Extrapolation from this regression equation yields a predicted group size for modern humans very similar to that of certain hunter-gatherer and traditional horticulturalist societies. Groups of similar size are also found in other large-scale forms of contemporary and historical society. Among primates, the cohesion of groups is maintained by social grooming; the time devoted to social grooming is linearly related to group size among the Old World monkeys and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   217 citations  
  • Genetic assimilation and a possible evolutionary paradox: can macroevolution sometimes be so fast to pass us by?Massimo Pigliucci - 2003 - Evolution 57 (7):1455-1464.
    The idea of genetic assimilation, that environmentally induced phenotypes may become genetically fixed and no longer require the original environmental stimulus, has had varied success through time in evolutionary biology research. Proposed by Waddington in the 1940s, it became an area of active empirical research mostly thanks to the efforts of its inventor and his collaborators. It was then attacked as of minor importance during the ‘‘hardening’’ of the neo-Darwinian synthesis and was relegated to a secondary role for decades. Recently, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • ‘‘Describing our whole experience’’: The statistical philosophies of W. F. R. Weldon and Karl Pearson.Charles H. Pence - 2011 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 42 (4):475-485.
    There are two motivations commonly ascribed to historical actors for taking up statistics: to reduce complicated data to a mean value (e.g., Quetelet), and to take account of diversity (e.g., Galton). Different motivations will, it is assumed, lead to different methodological decisions in the practice of the statistical sciences. Karl Pearson and W. F. R. Weldon are generally seen as following directly in Galton’s footsteps. I argue for two related theses in light of this standard interpretation, based on a reading (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Evolution and nursing.Trevor Hussey - 2002 - Nursing Philosophy 3 (3):240-251.
    Evolutionary theory has been a very popular topic in recent years and it has been claimed that it can make a major contribution to the advance of several sciences such as medicine, psychology, psychopathology and sociology: even providing them with new paradigms. This paper explores the possibility that nursing could benefit similarly by adopting an evolutionary perspective. After sketching the scientific and philosophical background to the recent developments concerning evolution, and briefly mentioning the chief features of evolutionary theory, the paper (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A Darwinian theory of cultural evolution can promote an evolutionary synthesis for the social sciences.Alex Mesoudi - 2007 - Biological Theory 2 (3):263-275.
    The evolutionary synthesis of the 1930s and 1940s integrated the study of biological microevolution and biological macroevolution into the theoretically consistent and hugely productive field of evolutionary biology. A similar synthesis has yet to occur for the study of culture, and the social sciences remain fragmented and theoretically incompatible. Here, it is suggested that a Darwinian theory of cultural evolution can promote such a synthesis. Earlier non-Darwinian theories of cultural evolution, such as progress theories, lacked key elements of a Darwinian (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Drift: A historical and conceptual overview.Anya Plutynski - 2007 - Biological Theory 2 (2):156-167.
    There are several different ways in which chance affects evolutionary change. That all of these processes are called “random genetic drift” is in part a due to common elements across these different processes, but is also a product of historical borrowing of models and language across different levels of organization in the biological hierarchy. A history of the concept of drift will reveal the variety of contexts in which drift has played an explanatory role in biology, and will shed light (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Species, demes, and the omega taxonomy: Gilmour and the newsystematics. [REVIEW]Mary Pickard Winsor - 2000 - Biology and Philosophy 15 (3):349-388.
    The word ``deme'' was coined by the botanists J.S.L. Gilmour and J.W.Gregor in 1939, following the pattern of J.S. Huxley's ``cline''. Its purposewas not only to rationalize the plethora of terms describing chromosomaland genetic variation, but also to reduce hostility between traditionaltaxonomists and researchers on evolution, who sometimes scorned eachother's understanding of species. A multi-layered system of compoundterms based on deme was published by Gilmour and J. Heslop-Harrison in1954 but not widely used. Deme was adopted with a modified meaning byzoologists (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Biological adaptation.Ronald Munson - 1971 - Philosophy of Science 38 (2):200-215.
    In this paper I attempt to show that adaptational sentences (i.e. sentences containing the terms "adaptive", "adapted", etc.) in evolutionary biology are best interpreted as equivalent to sentences about Darwinian or genetical selection. Thus, the use of adaptational languages does not introduce final purposes or other nonempirical notions into biology. I also try to demonstrate that adaptational sentences and functional sentences are not equivalent in an evolutionary context so that an analysis of function does not dispense with the need for (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • The modern synthesis and “Progress” in evolution: a view from the journal literature.Charles H. Pence - 2024 - History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences 46 (4):39.
    The concept of “progress” in evolutionary theory and its relationship to a putative notion of “Progress” in a global, normatively loaded sense of “change for the better” have been the subject of debate since Darwin admonished himself in a marginal note to avoid using the terms ‘higher’ and ‘lower.’ While an increase in some kind of complexity in the natural world might seem self-evident, efforts to explicate this trend meet notorious philosophical difficulties. Numerous historians pin the Modern Synthesis as a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A Kuhnian revolution in molecular biology: Most genes in complex organisms express regulatory RNAs.John S. Mattick - 2023 - Bioessays 45 (9):2300080.
    Thomas Kuhn described the progress of science as comprising occasional paradigm shifts separated by interludes of ‘normal science’. The paradigm that has held sway since the inception of molecular biology is that genes (mainly) encode proteins. In parallel, theoreticians posited that mutation is random, inferred that most of the genome in complex organisms is non‐functional, and asserted that somatic information is not communicated to the germline. However, many anomalies appeared, particularly in plants and animals: the strange genetic phenomena of paramutation (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Multiplicity of Research Programs in the Biological Systematics: A Case for Scientific Pluralism.Igor Y. Pavlinov - 2020 - Philosophies 5 (2):7.
    Biological diversity (BD) explored by biological systematics is a complex yet organized natural phenomenon and can be partitioned into several aspects, defined naturally with reference to various causal factors structuring biota. These BD aspects are studied by particular research programs based on specific taxonomic theories (TTs). They provide, in total, a framework for comprehending the structure of biological systematics and its multi-aspect relations to other fields of biology. General principles of individualizing BD aspects and construing TTs as quasi-axiomatics are briefly (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Extended Evolutionary Synthesis: Neither Synthesis Nor Extension.Claudio Ricardo Martins dos Reis & Leonardo Augusto Luvison Araújo - 2020 - Biological Theory 15 (2):57-60.
    The extended evolutionary synthesis intends to offer a new framework for understanding evolution based mainly on empirical and theoretical findings of current studies, including heredity and evolutionary developmental biology. In this essay, we present and develop the following objections about the terminology associated with the EES literature: despite using the term "extension," EES protagonists claim new evolutionary processes, reformulate conceptual networks, and modify central assumptions of the evolutionary synthesis. Therefore, the difference between ES and EES should not be described in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The Unfinished Synthesis?: Paleontology and Evolutionary Biology in the 20th Century.David Sepkoski - 2019 - Journal of the History of Biology 52 (4):687-703.
    In the received view of the history of the Modern Evolutionary Synthesis, paleontology was given a prominent role in evolutionary biology thanks to the significant influence of paleontologist George Gaylord Simpson on both the institutional and conceptual development of the Synthesis. Simpson's 1944 Tempo and Mode in Evolution is considered a classic of Synthesis-era biology, and Simpson often remarked on the influence of other major Synthesis figures – such as Ernst Mayr and Theodosius Dobzhansky – on his developing thought. Why, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • (1 other version)Die Architektur der Synthese. Entstehung und Philosophie der modernen Evolutionstheorie.Marcel Weber - 1996 - Dissertation, University of Konstanz
    This Ph.D. thesis provides a pilosophical account of the structure of the evolutionary synthesis of the 1930s and 40s. The first, more historical part analyses how classical genetics came to be integrated into evolutionary thinking, highlighting in particular the importance of chromosomal mapping of Drosophila strains collected in the wild by Dobzansky, but also the work of Goldschmidt, Sumners, Timofeeff-Ressovsky and others. The second, more philosophical part attempts to answer the question wherein the unity of the synthesis consisted. I argue (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Modern Synthesis is the Light of Microbial Genomics.Austin Booth, Carlos Mariscal & W. Ford Doolittle - 2016 - Annual Reviews of Microbiology 70 (1):279-297.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Group size, language and evolutionary mechanisms.Harold Kincaid - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):713-714.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The rest of the story: Grooming, group size and vocal exchanges in neotropical primates.Charles T. Snowdon - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):718-718.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Social complexity: The roles of primates' grooming and people's talking.Andrew Whiten - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):719-719.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Did primates need more than social grooming and increased group size for acquiring language?Jan Wind - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):720-720.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The functions of grooming and language: The present need not reflect the past.Marc Hauser, Leah Gardner, Tony Goldberg & Adrian Treves - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):706-707.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Another primate brain fiction: Brain (cortex) weight and homogeneity.Ralph L. Holloway - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):707-708.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Vocal grooming: Man the schmoozer.David Dean - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):699-700.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations