Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Building machines that learn and think like people.Brenden M. Lake, Tomer D. Ullman, Joshua B. Tenenbaum & Samuel J. Gershman - 2017 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40.
    Recent progress in artificial intelligence has renewed interest in building systems that learn and think like people. Many advances have come from using deep neural networks trained end-to-end in tasks such as object recognition, video games, and board games, achieving performance that equals or even beats that of humans in some respects. Despite their biological inspiration and performance achievements, these systems differ from human intelligence in crucial ways. We review progress in cognitive science suggesting that truly human-like learning and thinking (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   98 citations  
  • Philosophical foundations of the Death and Anti-Death discussion.Jeremy Horne - 2017 - Death And Anti-Death Set of Anthologies 15:72.
    Perhaps there has been no greater opportunity than in this “VOLUME FIFTEEN of our Death And Anti-Death set of anthologies” to write about how might think about life and how to avoid death. There are two reasons to discuss “life”, the first being enhancing our understanding of who we are and why we may be here in the Universe. The second is more practical: how humans meet the physical challenges brought about by the way they have interacted with their environment. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Imitation, Inspiration, and Creation: Cognitive Process of Creative Drawing by Copying Others' Artworks.Takeshi Okada & Kentaro Ishibashi - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (7):1804-1837.
    To investigate the cognitive processes underlying creative inspiration, we tested the extent to which viewing or copying prior examples impacted creative output in art. In Experiment 1, undergraduates made drawings under three conditions: copying an artist's drawing, then producing an original drawing; producing an original drawing without having seen another's work; and copying another artist's work, then reproducing that artist's style independently. We discovered that through copying unfamiliar abstract drawings, participants were able to produce creative drawings qualitatively different from the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Are species intelligent?: Not a yes or no question.Jonathan Schull - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):94-108.
    Plant and animal species are information-processing entities of such complexity, integration, and adaptive competence that it may be scientifically fruitful to consider them intelligent. The possibility arises from the analogy between learning and evolution, and from recent developments in evolutionary science, psychology and cognitive science. Species are now described as spatiotemporally localized individuals in an expanded hierarchy of biological entities. Intentional and cognitive abilities are now ascribed to animal, human, and artificial intelligence systems that process information adaptively, and that manifest (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Teaching an old dog new tricks.Daniel C. Dennett - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):76-77.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Species intelligence: Analogy without homology.James W. Kalat - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):80-80.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The risks of rationalising cognitive development.Beatrice de Gelder - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):713-714.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Transforming a partially structured brain into a creative mind.Annette Karmiloff-Smith - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):732-745.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Tales of the Unexpected: Incongruity-Resolution in Joke Comprehension, Scientific Discovery and Thought Experimentation.Tim De Mey - 2005 - Logic and Logical Philosophy 14 (1):69-88.
    Some scholars suspect that thought experiments have something in common with jokes. Moreover, Thomas Kuhn has suggested that what happens to someone who thinks through a thought experiment “is very similar to what happens to a man, like Lavoisier, who must assimilate the result of a new unexpected experimental discovery” (1964: 321). In this paper, I pinpoint the presumed commonalities. I identify, more specifically, what cognitive linguists call “incongruity-resolution” as the problem-solving process not only involved in humor comprehension, but in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Scientific discovery as a combinatorial optimisation problem: How best to navigate the landscape of possible experiments?Douglas B. Kell - 2012 - Bioessays 34 (3):236-244.
    A considerable number of areas of bioscience, including gene and drug discovery, metabolic engineering for the biotechnological improvement of organisms, and the processes of natural and directed evolution, are best viewed in terms of a ‘landscape’ representing a large search space of possible solutions or experiments populated by a considerably smaller number of actual solutions that then emerge. This is what makes these problems ‘hard’, but as such these are to be seen as combinatorial optimisation problems that are best attacked (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Understanding Biological Mechanisms: Using Illustrations from Circadian Rhythm Research.William Bechtel - unknown
    In many fields of biology, researchers explain a phenomenon by characterizing the responsible mechanism. This requires identifying the candidate mechanism, decomposing it into its parts and operations, recomposing it so as to understand how it is organized and its operations orchestrated to generate the phenomenon, and situating it in its environment. Mechanistic researchers have developed sophisticated tools for decomposing mechanisms but new approaches, including modeling, are increasingly being invoked to recompose mechanisms when they involve nonsequential organization of nonlinear operations. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Interpreting scientific and engineering practices: Integrating the cognitive, social, and cultural dimensions.N. J. Nersessian - 2005 - In M. Gorman, R. Tweney, D. Gooding & A. Kincannon (eds.), Scientific and Technological Thinking. Erlbaum. pp. 17--56.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • Constructing a Philosophy of Science of Cognitive Science.William Bechtel - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (3):548-569.
    Philosophy of science is positioned to make distinctive contributions to cognitive science by providing perspective on its conceptual foundations and by advancing normative recommendations. The philosophy of science I embrace is naturalistic in that it is grounded in the study of actual science. Focusing on explanation, I describe the recent development of a mechanistic philosophy of science from which I draw three normative consequences for cognitive science. First, insofar as cognitive mechanisms are information-processing mechanisms, cognitive science needs an account of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Human creativity: Its cognitive basis, its evolution, and its connections with childhood pretence.Peter Carruthers - 2002 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 53 (2):225-249.
    This paper defends two initial claims. First, it argues that essentially the same cognitive resources are shared by adult creative thinking and problem-solving, on the one hand, and by childhood pretend play, on the other—namely, capacities to generate and to reason with suppositions (or imagined possibilities). Second, it argues that the evolutionary function of childhood pretence is to practice and enhance adult forms of creativity. The paper goes on to show how these proposals can provide a smooth and evolutionarily-plausible explanation (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Patterns of abduction.Gerhard Schurz - 2008 - Synthese 164 (2):201-234.
    This article describes abductions as special patterns of inference to the best explanation whose structure determines a particularly promising abductive conjecture and thus serves as an abductive search strategy. A classification of different patterns of abduction is provided which intends to be as complete as possible. An important distinction is that between selective abductions, which choose an optimal candidate from given multitude of possible explanations, and creative abductions, which introduce new theoretical models or concepts. While selective abduction has dominated the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   95 citations  
  • Scientific discovery, causal explanation, and process model induction.Pat Langley - 2019 - Mind and Society 18 (1):43-56.
    In this paper, I review two related lines of computational research: discovery of scientific knowledge and causal models of scientific phenomena. I also report research on quantitative process models that falls at the intersection of these two themes. This framework represents models as a set of interacting processes, each with associated differential equations that express influences among variables. Simulating such a quantitative process model produces trajectories for variables over time that one can compare to observations. Background knowledge about candidate processes (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Heuristics and Inferential Microstructures: The Path to Quaternions.Emiliano Ippoliti - 2019 - Foundations of Science 24 (3):411-425.
    I investigate the construction of the mathematical concept of quaternion from a methodological and heuristic viewpoint to examine what we can learn from it for the study of the advancement of mathematical knowledge. I will look, in particular, at the inferential microstructures that shape this construction, that is, the study of both the very first, ampliative inferential steps, and their tentative outcomes—i.e. small ‘structures’ such as provisional entities and relations. I discuss how this paradigmatic case study supports the recent approaches (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Naturalizing Epistemology: Quine, Simon and the Prospects for Pragmatism.Stephen Stich - 1993 - Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement 34:1-17.
    In recent years there has been a great deal of discussion about the prospects of developing a ‘naturalized epistemology’, though different authors tend to interpret this label in quite different ways. One goal of this paper is to sketch three projects that might lay claim to the ‘naturalized epistemology’ label, and to argue that they are not all equally attractive. Indeed, I'll maintain that the first of the three—the one I'll attribute to Quine—is simply incoherent. There is no way we (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Representational redescription and cognitive architectures.Antonella Carassa & Maurizio Tirassa - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):711-712.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Implicit and explicit representations of time.John A. Michon - 1990 - In Richard A. Block (ed.), Cognitive Models of Psychological Time. Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 37--58.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Why would we ever doubt that species are intelligent?Nicholas S. Thompson - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):94-94.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Biotic intelligence (BI)?F. J. Odling-Smee - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):83-84.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Which came first, the egg-problem or the hen-solution?Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):84-86.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • “Intelligence” as description and as explanation.P. A. Russell - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):86-86.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Misplaced predicates and misconstrued intelligence.Stanley N. Salthe - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):86-87.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Natural teleology and species intelligence.Albert Silverstein - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):87-89.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are species intelligent? Look for genetic knowledge structures.J. David Smith - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):89-90.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning, selection, and species.Kim Sterelny - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):90-91.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Of cockroaches as kings.Robert J. Sternberg - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):91-91.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neo-Lamarckism, or, The rediscovery of culture.Gary W. Strong - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):92-93.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Effective search using Sewall Wright's shifting balance hypothesis.B. H. Sumida - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):93-93.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The ontology of “intelligent species”.Philip Clayton - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):75-76.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are species Gaia's thoughts?V. Csányi - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):76-76.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unfortunately, scale and time matter.Kim C. Derrickson & Russell S. Greenberg - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):77-78.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are libraries intelligent?Michael T. Ghiselin - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):78-78.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Species intelligence: Hazards of structural parallels.Robert W. Hendersen - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):78-79.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Similarities and dissimilarities between adaptation and learning.Mark H. Johnson - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):79-80.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Evolution, development, and learning in cognitive science.David Leiser - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):80-81.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “Intelligent” evolution and neo-Darwinian straw men.Elisabeth A. Lloyd - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):81-82.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The way of all matter.William A. MacKay - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):82-83.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Are species intelligent?: Not a yes or no question.Jonathan Schull - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):63-75.
    Plant and animal species are information-processing entities of such complexity, integration, and adaptive competence that it may be scientifically fruitful to consider them intelligent. The possibility arises from the analogy between learning and evolution, and from recent developments in evolutionary science, psychology and cognitive science. Species are now described as spatiotemporally localized individuals in an expanded hierarchy of biological entities. Intentional and cognitive abilities are now ascribed to animal, human, and artificial intelligence systems that process information adaptively, and that manifest (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Beyond modularity: Neural evidence for constructivist principles in development.Steven R. Quartz & Terrence J. Sejnowski - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):725-726.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Where redescriptions come from.David R. Olson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):725-725.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The power of explicit knowing.Deanna Kuhn - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):722-723.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Beyond methodological solipsism?Michael Losonsky - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):723-724.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Genes, development, and the “innate” structure of the mind.Timothy D. Johnston - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):721-722.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Representational redescription, memory, and connectionism.P. J. Hampson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):721-721.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Dissociation, self-attribution, and redescription.George Graham - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):719-719.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Beyond connectionist versus classical Al: A control theoretic perspective on development and cognitive science.Rick Grush - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):720-720.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do you have to be right to redescribe?Susan Goldin-Meadow & Martha Wagner Alibali - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):718-719.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark