Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. What are the chemical characteristics of brain mechanisms for aggression?Klaus A. Miczek - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):224-225.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Motives: Metaphors in motion.John C. Fentress - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):219-219.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The motivation and/or reinforcement of avoidance behavior.Robert C. Bolles - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):677-678.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the nature of specific hard-wired brain circuits.Allan Siegel - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):443-444.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Emotions are objective events.Elzbieta Fonberg - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):429-430.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Can phenomenology contribute to brain science?Gordon G. Globus - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):430-431.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Parting's sweet sorrow: A pain pathway for the social sentiments?Leonard D. Katz - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):435-436.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Panic, separation anxiety, and endorphins.Donald F. Klein - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):436-437.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Assessing internal affairs.Hymie Anisman & Robert M. Zacharko - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):422-423.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • They are really complex when you get to know them.Irving Kupfermann - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):393-394.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • We are making good progress in the neural analysis of behaviour.David L. Macmillan - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):395-395.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Points of congruence between ethology and neuroscience.Wolfgang M. Schleidt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):398-399.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can neuroethologists be led?Fred Delcomyn - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):385.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Intentions as goads.David McFarland - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):369-370.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • International plovers or just dump brids?Carolyn A. Ristau - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):373-375.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Adaptationism was always predictive and needed no defense.Richard Dawkins - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):360-361.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Biosemiotics: Its roots, proliferation, and prospects.Thomas A. Sebeok - 2001 - Semiotica 2001 (134).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   59 citations  
  • Cuteness and Disgust: The Humanizing and Dehumanizing Effects of Emotion.Gary D. Sherman & Jonathan Haidt - 2011 - Emotion Review 3 (3):245-251.
    Moral emotions are evolved mechanisms that function in part to optimize social relationships. We discuss two moral emotions— disgust and the “cuteness response”—which modulate social-engagement motives in opposite directions, changing the degree to which the eliciting entity is imbued with mental states (i.e., mentalized). Disgust-inducing entities are hypo-mentalized (i.e., dehumanized); cute entities are hyper-mentalized (i.e., “humanized”). This view of cuteness—which challenges the prevailing view that cuteness is a releaser of parental instincts (Lorenz, 1950/1971)—explains (a) the broad range of affiliative behaviors (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • The Strangest Sort of Map: Reply to Commentaries.Stephen Asma - 2021 - Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture 5 (2):75-82.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Institution of Life in Gehlen and Merleau-Ponty: Searching for the Common Ground for the Anthropological Difference.Jan Halák & Jiří Klouda - 2018 - Human Studies 41 (3):371-394.
    The goal of our article is to review the widespread anthropological figure, according to which we can achieve a better understanding of humans by contrasting them with animals. This originally Herderian approach was elaborated by Arnold Gehlen, who characterized humans as “deficient beings” who become complete through culture. According to Gehlen, humans, who are insufficiently equipped by instincts, indirectly stabilize their existence by creating institutions, i.e., complexes of habitual actions. On the other hand, Maurice Merleau-Ponty shows that corporeal relationship to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • A teleofunctional account of evolutionary mismatch.Nathan Cofnas - 2016 - Biology and Philosophy 31 (4):507-525.
    When the environment in which an organism lives deviates in some essential way from that to which it is adapted, this is described as “evolutionary mismatch,” or “evolutionary novelty.” The notion of mismatch plays an important role, explicitly or implicitly, in evolution-informed cognitive psychology, clinical psychology, and medicine. The evolutionary novelty of our contemporary environment is thought to have significant implications for our health and well-being. However, scientists have generally been working without a clear definition of mismatch. This paper defines (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Is there anything new in the neurophysiology of aggression for social psychologists?Adam Fraczek - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):219-220.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neurotransmitter organization of aggressive behavior.László Decsi & Julia Nagy - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):216-217.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Birdsong: Variations that follow rules.Dietmar Todt & Henrike Hultsch - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):289-290.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the classification of the emotions.Jeffrey A. Gray - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):431-432.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • From stimulus-bound emotive command systems to drive-free emotions.C. E. Izard - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):433-434.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A two-tiered theory of emotions: Affect and feeling.Julian Jaynes - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):434-435.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • Denoting and demoting international systems.George Graham - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):363-364.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Biological roots of musical epistemology: Functional cycles, Umwelt, and enactive listening.Mark Reybrouck - 2001 - Semiotica 2001 (134):599-633.
    This article argues for an epistemology of music, stating that dealing with music can be considered as a process of knowledge acquisition. What really matters is not the representation of an ontological musical reality, but the generation of music knowledge as a tool for adaptation to the sonic world. Three major positions are brought together: the epistemological claims of Jean Piaget, the biological methodology of Jakob von Uexküll, and the constructivistic conceptions of Ernst von Glasersfeld, each ingstress the role of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Ecosemiotics and the sustainability transition.Soren Brier - 2001 - Sign Systems Studies 29 (1):219-234.
    The emerging epistemic community of ecosemioticians and the multidisciplinary field of inquiry known as ecosemiotics offer a radical and relevant approach to so-called global environmental crisis. There are no environmental fixes within the dominant code, since that code overdetermines the future, thereby perpetuating ecologically untenable cultural forms. The possibility of a sustainability transition (the attempt to overcome destitution and avoid ecocatastrophe) becomes real when mediated by and through ecosemiotics. In short, reflexive awareness of humankind's linguisticality is a necessary condition for (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What Is Ineffable?Jan Zwicky - 2012 - International Studies in the Philosophy of Science 26 (2):197-217.
    In this essay, I argue, via a revision of Freud's notions of primary and secondary process, that experiences of resonant form lie at the root of many serious ineffability claims. I suggest further that Western European culture's resistance to the perception of resonant form underlies some of its present crises.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Pretense and Display Theories of Theatrical Performance.James R. Hamilton - 2009 - Organon F: Medzinárodný Časopis Pre Analytickú Filozofiu (4):632-654.
    A survey of and a comparison of the relative strengths of two favored views of what theatrical performers do: pretend or engage in a variety of self-display. The behavioral version of the pretense theory is shown to be relatively weak as an instrument for understanding the variety of performance styles available in world theater. Whether pretense works as a theory of the mental capacities that underly theatrical performance is a separate question.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Self-other organization: Why early life did not evolve through natural selection.Liane Gabora - manuscript
    The improbability of a spontaneously generated self-assembling molecule has suggested that life began with a set of simpler, collectively replicating elements, such as an enclosed autocatalytic set of polymers (or autocell). Since replication occurs without a self-assembly code, acquired characteristics are inherited. Moreover, there is no strict distinction between alive and dead; one can only infer that an autocell was alive if it replicates. These features of early life render natural selection inapplicable to the description of its change-of-state because they (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Towards welfare biology: Evolutionary economics of animal consciousness and suffering. [REVIEW]Yew-Kwang Ng - 1995 - Biology and Philosophy 10 (3):255-285.
    Welfare biology is the study of living things and their environment with respect to their welfare. Despite difficulties of ascertaining and measuring welfare and relevancy to normative issues, welfare biology is a positive science. Evolutionary economics and population dynamics are used to help answer basic questions in welfare biology : Which species are affective sentients capable of welfare? Do they enjoy positive or negative welfare? Can their welfare be dramatically increased? Under plausible axioms, all conscious species are plastic and all (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   50 citations  
  • Ideas are not replicators but minds are.Liane Gabora - 2004 - Biology and Philosophy 19 (1):127-143.
    An idea is not a replicator because it does not consist of coded self-assembly instructions. It may retain structure as it passes from one individual to another, but does not replicate it. The cultural replicator is not an idea but an associatively-structured network of them that together form an internal model of the world, or worldview. A worldview is a primitive, uncoded replicator, like the autocatalytic sets of polymers widely believed to be the earliest form of life. Primitive replicators generate (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Naturalistic ethics: Problem in reductionism.John Cassidy - 1978 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 8 (2):193–216.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Aesthetics or Communication?: Social Semiotic Traits of Structured Forms in Studies of “Animal Beauty”.Sigmund Ongstad - forthcoming - Biosemiotics:1-24.
    The article investigates basic relations between aesthetics and communication based on studies of and discussions about what has been termed “animal beauty”. The concepts _beauty_, _aesthetics_, and _communication_ are problematised, starting from utterances’ _structured form_, which is seen both as the physical basis for as well as one of five key aspects in animal utterances (form, content, act, time, and space). The relational, and thus social semiotic, communicational role of this aspect is searched in different studies leading to two major (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • White Skin Privilege: Modern Myth, Forgotten Past.Peter Frost - 2020 - Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture 4 (2):63-82.
    European women dominate images of beauty, presumably because Europe has dominated the world for the past few centuries. Yet this presumed cause poorly explains “white slavery”-the commodification of European women for export at a time when their continent was much less dominant. Actually, there has long been a cross-cultural preference for lighter-skinned women, with the notable exception of modern Western culture. This cultural norm mirrors a physical norm: skin sexually differentiates at puberty, becoming fairer in girls, and browner and ruddier (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From the Hiatus Model to the Diffuse Discontinuities: A Turning Point in Human-Animal Studies.Carlo Brentari - 2018 - Biosemiotics 11 (3):331-345.
    In twentieth-century continental philosophy, German philosophical anthropology can be seen as a sort of conceptual laboratory devoted to human/animal research, and, in particular, to the discontinuity between human and non-human animals. Its main notion—the idea of the special position of humans in nature—is one of the first philosophical attempts to think of the specificity of humans as a natural and qualitative difference from non-human animals. This school of thought correctly rejects both the metaphysical and/or religious characterisations of humans, and the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • From psychopharmacology to neuropsychopharmacology: Adapting behavioral terminology to neural events.George V. Rebec - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):287-288.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Moving beyond words.Robert Fagen - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):275-276.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Connecting invertebrate behavior, neurophysiology and evolution with Eshkol-Wachman movement notation.Zen Faulkes & Dorothy Hayman Paul - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):276-277.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dynamical systems theory and the mobility gradient: Information, homology and self-similar structure.Gary Goldberg - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):278-279.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neuroethology: A call for less exclusivity and more theory.Michael A. Arbib - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):381.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Elementary errors about evolution.Richard C. Lewontin - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):367-368.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   60 citations  
  • Lloyd Morgan's canon in evolutionary context.Michael T. Ghiselin - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):362-363.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   63 citations  
  • Thinking about animal thoughts.Donald R. Griffin - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):364-364.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • Is the Mauthner cell a Kupfermann & Weiss command neuron?Robert C. Eaton, Chris M. Wieland & Randolf DiDomenico - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):725-727.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • Reinventing molecular weismannism: Information in evolution. [REVIEW]James MacLaurin - 1998 - Biology and Philosophy 13 (1):37-59.
    Molecular Weismannism is the claim that: “In the development of an individual, DNA causes the production both of DNA (genetic material) and of protein (somatic material). The reverse process never occurs. Protein is never a cause of DNA”. This principle underpins both the idea that genes are the objects upon which natural selection operates and the idea that traits can be divided into those that are genetic and those that are not. Recent work in developmental biology and in philosophy of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Modelling, dialogism and the functional cycle.Susan Petrilli & Augusto Ponzio - 2013 - Sign Systems Studies 41 (1):93-113.
    Charles Peirce, Mikhail Bakhtin and Thomas Sebeok all develop original research itineraries around the sign and, despite terminological differences, canbe related with reference to the concept of dialogism and modelling. Jakob von Uexküll’s biosemiosic “functional cycle”, a model for semiosic processes, is alsoimplied in the relation between dialogue and communication.Biological models which describe communication as a self-referential, autopoietic and semiotically closed system (e.g., the models proposed by Maturana,Varela, and Thure von Uexküll) contrast with both the linear (Shannon and Weaver) and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations