Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Turing's golden: How well Turing's work stands today.Justin Leiber - 2006 - Philosophical Psychology 19 (1):13-46.
    A. M. Turing has bequeathed us a conceptulary including 'Turing, or Turing-Church, thesis', 'Turing machine', 'universal Turing machine', 'Turing test' and 'Turing structures', plus other unnamed achievements. These include a proof that any formal language adequate to express arithmetic contains undecidable formulas, as well as achievements in computer science, artificial intelligence, mathematics, biology, and cognitive science. Here it is argued that these achievements hang together and have prospered well in the 50 years since Turing's death.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Nonmonotonic reasoning by inhibition nets☆☆This paper has been supported by the Austrian Research Fund FWF (SFB F012).Hannes Leitgeb - 2001 - Artificial Intelligence 128 (1-2):161-201.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Values Evolution in Human Machine Relations: Grounding Computationalism and Neural Dynamics in a Physical a Priorism of Nature.Denis Larrivee - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15:649544.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pessimism, models, and episodic behavior.James L. Larimer & Wesley Thompson - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):554-555.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Generality and applications.Jill H. Larkin - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):486-487.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The gap from sensation to cognition.Michael S. Landy - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):101-102.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Approaches to learning and representation.Pat Langley - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):500-501.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What can psychologists learn from hidden-unit nets?K. Lamberts & G. D'Ydewalle - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):499-500.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Jakob von Uexküll and the origins of cybernetics.Kari Y. H. Lagerspetz - 2001 - Semiotica 2001 (134).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • They are really complex when you get to know them.Irving Kupfermann - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):393-394.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Deep-Learning-Based Multivariate Pattern Analysis (dMVPA): A Tutorial and a Toolbox.Karl M. Kuntzelman, Jacob M. Williams, Phui Cheng Lim, Ashok Samal, Prahalada K. Rao & Matthew R. Johnson - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    In recent years, multivariate pattern analysis has been hugely beneficial for cognitive neuroscience by making new experiment designs possible and by increasing the inferential power of functional magnetic resonance imaging, electroencephalography, and other neuroimaging methodologies. In a similar time frame, “deep learning” has produced a parallel revolution in the field of machine learning and has been employed across a wide variety of applications. Traditional MVPA also uses a form of machine learning, but most commonly with much simpler techniques based on (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How connectionist models learn: The course of learning in connectionist networks.John K. Kruschke - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):498-499.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Why does the human brain need to be a nonlinear system?Zbigniew J. Kowalik, Andrzej Wrobel & Andrzej Rydz - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):302-303.
    We focus on one aspect of Wright & Liley's target article: the linearity of the EEG. According to the authors, some nonlinear models of the cortex can be reduced (approximated) to the linear case at the millimetric scale. We argue here that the statement about the linear character of EEG is too strong and that EEG exhibits nonlinear features which cannot be ignored.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The how, what, and why of mental imagery.Stephen M. Kossyln, Steven Pinker, George E. Smith & Steven P. Shwartz - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):570-581.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • On the demystification of mental imagery.Stephen M. Kosslyn, Steven Pinker, George E. Smith & Steven P. Shwartz - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):535-548.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   196 citations  
  • On the demystification of mental imagery.Stephen M. Kosslyn, Steven Pinker, Sophie Schwartz & G. Smith - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):535-81.
    What might a theory of mental imagery look like, and how might one begin formulating such a theory? These are the central questions addressed in the present paper. The first section outlines the general research direction taken here and provides an overview of the empirical foundations of our theory of image representation and processing. Four issues are considered in succession, and the relevant results of experiments are presented and discussed. The second section begins with a discussion of the proper form (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   200 citations  
  • Comparative reduction of theories — or over-simplification?Edgar Koerner - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):301-302.
    To model the organization of levels' of cortical dynamics, at least some general scheme for hierarchy, functional diversity, and proper intrinsic control must be provided. Rhythmic control forces the system to iterate its state by short trajectories, which makes it much more stable and predictable without discarding the desirable ability of chaotic systems to make rapid phase transitions. Rhythmic control provides a fundamentally different systems dynamics, one not provided by models that allow the emergence of continuous trajectories in the systems (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Quantum Concept of Consciousness: For or Against?Victor N. Knyazev & Galina V. Parshikova - 2023 - RUDN Journal of Philosophy 27 (4):901-914.
    The study examines a problematic hypothesis of possible approaches to identifying the quantum physical foundations of the functioning of consciousness. The authors proceed from the fact that in modern conditions, not a single science, nor all sciences taken together, gives a final answer to the question of the “mechanism” of the origin of thought. However, this does not mean at all that research in this direction needs to be stopped. The authors express confidence that modern and subsequent research into the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Underestimating the importance of the implementational level.Michael Van Kleeck - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):497-498.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The imagery debate: a controversy over terms and cognitive styles.Janice M. Keenan & Richard K. Olson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):558-559.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Comparison Between Kanerva's SDM and Hopfield‐type Neural Networks.James D. Keeler - 1988 - Cognitive Science 12 (3):299-329.
    The Sparse, Distributed Memory (SDM) model (Kanerva, 1984) is compared to Hopfield-type, neural-network models. A mathematical framework for comparing the two models is developed, and the capacity of each model is investigated. The capacity of the SDM can be increased independent of the dimension of the stored vectors, whereas the Hopfield capacity is limited to a fraction of this dimension. The stored information is proportional to the number of connections, and it is shown that this proportionality constant is the same (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A practical approach to understanding central pattern generators.C. R. S. Kaneko - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):554-554.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A non-empiricist perspective on learning in layered networks.Michael I. Jordan - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):497-498.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The “thoughtless imagery” controversy.P. N. Johnson-Laird - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):557-558.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A Brief Review of Neural Networks Based Learning and Control and Their Applications for Robots.Yiming Jiang, Chenguang Yang, Jing Na, Guang Li, Yanan Li & Junpei Zhong - 2017 - Complexity:1-14.
    As an imitation of the biological nervous systems, neural networks, which have been characterized as powerful learning tools, are employed in a wide range of applications, such as control of complex nonlinear systems, optimization, system identification, and patterns recognition. This article aims to bring a brief review of the state-of-the-art NNs for the complex nonlinear systems by summarizing recent progress of NNs in both theory and practical applications. Specifically, this survey also reviews a number of NN based robot control algorithms, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Vertebrate neuroethology: Doomed from the start?David J. Ingle - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):392-393.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Nonlinear nonequilibrium nonquantum nonchaotic statistical mechanics of neocortical interactions.Lester Ingber - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):300-301.
    The work in progress reported by Wright & Liley shows great promise, primarily because of their experimental and simulation paradigms. However, their tentative conclusion that macroscopic neocortex may be considered (approximately) a linear near-equilibrium system is premature and does not correspond to tentative conclusions drawn from other studies of neocortex.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mental visualization in nonlaboratory situations.Ian M. L. Hunter - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):556-557.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neuroethology, according to Hoyle.Franz Huber - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):391-392.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Central pattern generators from the viewpoint of a behavioral physiologist.Franz Huber - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):553-554.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The scope of neuroethology.Graham Hoyle - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):367.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   59 citations  
  • Neuroethology: To be, or not to be?Graham Hoyle - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):403-412.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Expectation and achievement in analysis of motor program generation.Graham Hoyle - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):552-553.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mathematical biophysics and the central nervous system.Alston S. Householder - 1946 - Acta Biotheoretica 8 (1-2):67-76.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • “Grandmother networks” and computational economy.J. J. Hopfield - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):100-100.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Invariant and programmable neuropsychological systems are fibrations.William C. Hoffman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):99-100.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognition Without Neural Representation: Dynamics of a Complex System.Inês Hipólito - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    This paper proposes an account of neurocognitive activity without leveraging the notion of neural representation. Neural representation is a concept that results from assuming that the properties of the models used in computational cognitive neuroscience must literally exist the system being modelled. Computational models are important tools to test a theory about how the collected data has been generated. While the usefulness of computational models is unquestionable, it does not follow that neurocognitive activity should literally entail the properties construed in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Imagery without arrays.Geoffrey Hinton - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):555-556.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Ethology has progressed.Robert A. Hinde - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):391-391.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • But what is the substance of connectionist representation?James Hendler - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):496-497.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A flawed analogy?James Hendler - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):485-486.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mental Imagery and mystification.John Hell - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):554-555.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ontologies and Worlds in Category Theory: Implications for Neural Systems.Michael John Healy & Thomas Preston Caudell - 2006 - Axiomathes 16 (1-2):165-214.
    We propose category theory, the mathematical theory of structure, as a vehicle for defining ontologies in an unambiguous language with analytical and constructive features. Specifically, we apply categorical logic and model theory, based upon viewing an ontology as a sub-category of a category of theories expressed in a formal logic. In addition to providing mathematical rigor, this approach has several advantages. It allows the incremental analysis of ontologies by basing them in an interconnected hierarchy of theories, with an operation on (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Understanding mental imagery: interpretive metaphors versus explanatory models.Frederick Hayes-Roth - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):553-554.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What we don't know about brains.Valerie Gray Hardcastle - 1999 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 30 (1):69-89.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The methodological role of mechanistic-computational models in cognitive science.Jens Harbecke - 2020 - Synthese 199 (Suppl 1):19-41.
    This paper discusses the relevance of models for cognitive science that integrate mechanistic and computational aspects. Its main hypothesis is that a model of a cognitive system is satisfactory and explanatory to the extent that it bridges phenomena at multiple mechanistic levels, such that at least several of these mechanistic levels are shown to implement computational processes. The relevant parts of the computation must be mapped onto distinguishable entities and activities of the mechanism. The ideal is contrasted with two other (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Roles for models in understanding neural networks.Daniel K. Hartline - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):551-552.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Must neural mechanisms be Newtonian?Erich Harth - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):550-551.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modeling for modeling's sake?Valerie Gray Hardcastle - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):299-299.
    Although this is an impressive piece of modeling work, I worry that the two models that Wright & Liley have created do not yet provide us with useful empirical information regarding brain processing.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does the brain compute?Erich Harth - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):98-99.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations