Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Computation and cognition: Issues in the foundation of cognitive science.Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):111-32.
    The computational view of mind rests on certain intuitions regarding the fundamental similarity between computation and cognition. We examine some of these intuitions and suggest that they derive from the fact that computers and human organisms are both physical systems whose behavior is correctly described as being governed by rules acting on symbolic representations. Some of the implications of this view are discussed. It is suggested that a fundamental hypothesis of this approach is that there is a natural domain of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   667 citations  
  • The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery.Marc Jeannerod - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):187-202.
    This paper concerns how motor actions are neurally represented and coded. Action planning and motor preparation can be studied using a specific type of representational activity, motor imagery. A close functional equivalence between motor imagery and motor preparation is suggested by the positive effects of imagining movements on motor learning, the similarity between the neural structures involved, and the similar physiological correlates observed in both imaging and preparing. The content of motor representations can be inferred from motor images at a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   358 citations  
  • The codes of man and beasts.David Premack - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):125-136.
    Exposing the chimpanzee to language training appears to enhance the animal's ability to perform some kinds of tasks but not others. The abilities that are enhanced involve abstract judgment, as in analogical reasoning, matching proportions of physically unlike exemplars, and completing incomplete representations of action. The abilities that do not improve concern the location of items in space and the inferences one might make in attempting to obtain them. Representing items in space and making inferences about them could be done (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   191 citations  
  • Précis of O'Keefe & Nadel's The hippocampus as a cognitive map.John O'Keefe & Lynn Nadel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):487-494.
    Theories of spatial cognition are derived from many sources. Psychologists are concerned with determining the features of the mind which, in combination with external inputs, produce our spatialized experience. A review of philosophical and other approaches has convinced us that the brain must come equipped to impose a three-dimensional Euclidean framework on experience – our analysis suggests that object re-identification may require such a framework. We identify this absolute, nonegocentric, spatial framework with a specific neural system centered in the hippocampus.A (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   180 citations  
  • The role of location indexes in spatial perception: A sketch of the FINST spatial-index model.Zenon Pylyshyn - 1989 - Cognition 32 (1):65-97.
    Marr (1982) may have been one of the rst vision researchers to insist that in modeling vision it is important to separate the location of visual features from their type. He argued that in early stages of visual processing there must be “place tokens” that enable subsequent stages of the visual system to treat locations independent of what specic feature type was at that location. Thus, in certain respects a collinear array of diverse features could still be perceived as a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   173 citations  
  • Mental imagery: In search of a theory.Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (2):157-182.
    It is generally accepted that there is something special about reasoning by using mental images. The question of how it is special, however, has never been satisfactorily spelled out, despite more than thirty years of research in the post-behaviorist tradition. This article considers some of the general motivation for the assumption that entertaining mental images involves inspecting a picture-like object. It sets out a distinction between phenomena attributable to the nature of mind to what is called the cognitive architecture, and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   127 citations  
  • Visual cognition: An introduction.Steven Pinker - 1984 - Cognition 18 (1-3):1-63.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   115 citations  
  • Cognitive representation and the process-architecture distinction.Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):154-169.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   78 citations  
  • Twenty years of haunting eidetic imagery: where's the ghost?Ralph Norman Haber - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):583-594.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   71 citations  
  • Précis of Gallistel's The organization of action: A new synthesis.C. R. Gallistel - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):609-619.
    The book describes three elementary units of action – the reflex, the oscillator, and the servomechanism – and the principles by which they are combined to make complex units. The combining of elementary units to make complex units gives behavior and the neural circuitry underlying behavior a hierarchical structure. Circuits at higher levels govern the operation of lower circuits by selective potentiation and depotentiation: by regulating the potential for operation in lower circuits – raising the potential for some and lowering (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   64 citations  
  • The cognitive map as a hippocampus.John O'Keefe & Lynn Nadel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):520-533.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   62 citations  
  • Imagery.Zenon Pylyshyn - 2004 - In Richard Langton Gregory, The Oxford companion to the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
    In Gregory, Richard. Oxford Companion to the Mind (Second Edition, 2006) Oxford University Press.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   60 citations  
  • A new abstract code or the new possibility of multiple codes?Annette Karmiloff Smith - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):149-150.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   59 citations  
  • A Cognitive Theory of Graphical and Linguistic Reasoning: Logic and Implementation.Keith Stenning & Jon Oberlander - 1995 - Cognitive Science 19 (1):97-140.
    We discuss external and internal graphical and linguistic representational systems. We argue that a cognitive theory of peoples' reasoning performance must account for (a) the logical equivalence of inferences expressed in graphical and linguistic form, and (b) the implementational differences that affect facility of inference. Our theory proposes that graphical representation limit abstraction and thereby aid “processibility”. We discuss the ideas of specificity and abstraction, and their cognitive relevance. Empirical support both comes from tasks which involve the manipulation of external (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • S eeingand visualizing: I T' S n otwhaty ou T hink.Zenon Pylyshyn - unknown
    6. Seeing With the Mind’s Eye 1: The Puzzle of Mental Imagery .................................................6-1 6.1 What is the puzzle about mental imagery?..............................................................................6-1 6.2 Content, form and substance of representations ......................................................................6-6 6.3 What is responsible for the pattern of results obtained in imagery studies?.................................6-8..
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  • Mental imagery.Nigel J. T. Thomas - 2001 - Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
    Mental imagery (varieties of which are sometimes colloquially refered to as “visualizing,” “seeing in the mind's eye,” “hearing in the head,” “imagining the feel of,” etc.) is quasi-perceptual experience; it resembles perceptual experience, but occurs in the absence of the appropriate external stimuli. It is also generally understood to bear intentionality (i.e., mental images are always images of something or other), and thereby to function as a form of mental representation. Traditionally, visual mental imagery, the most discussed variety, was thought (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Non-commitment in mental imagery.Eric J. Bigelow, John P. McCoy & Tomer D. Ullman - 2023 - Cognition 238 (C):105498.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Jeannerod's representing brain: Image or illusion?Jean Pailhous & Mireille Bonnard - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):215-216.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Imagery in action. G. H. Mead’s contribution to sensorimotor enactivism.Guido Baggio - 2021 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 20 (5):935-955.
    The aim of the article is to outline several valuable elements of Mead’s pragmatist theory of perception in action developed in his The Philosophy of the Act, in order to strengthen the pragmatist legacy of the enactivist approach. In particular, Mead’s theory of perception in action turns out to be a forerunner of sensorimotor enactivist theory. Unlike the latter, however, Mead explicitly refers to imagery as an essential capacity for agency. Nonetheless, the article argues that the ways in which Mead (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Some primitive mechanisms of spatial attention.Zenon Pylyshyn - 1994 - Cognition 50 (1-3):363-384.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • Spatial mapping only a special case of hippocampal function.Jeffrey A. Gray - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):501-503.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Analogical representations of naive physics.Francesco Gardin & Bernard Meltzer - 1989 - Artificial Intelligence 38 (2):139-159.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Waves and cells, maps and memories, space and time.J. Eric Holmes - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):505-506.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Visual perspective-taking and image-like representations: We don't see it.Steven Samuel, Klara Hagspiel, Madeline J. Eacott & Geoff G. Cole - 2021 - Cognition 210 (C):104607.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • (1 other version)The imagery debate.Kim Sterelny - 1986 - Philosophy of Science 53 (December):560-83.
    One central debate in cognitive science is over imagery. Do images constitute, or constitute evidence for, a distinctive, depictive form of mental representation? The most sophisticated advocacy of this view has been developed by Kosslyn and his coworkers. This paper focuses on his position and argues (i) that though Kosslyn has not developed a satisfactory account of depiction, there is nothing in principle unintelligible about the idea of depictive neural representation, but (ii) Kosslyn's model of imagery rescues the intelligibility of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • The tripartite model of representation.Peter Slezak - 2002 - Philosophical Psychology 15 (3):239-270.
    Robert Cummins [(1996) Representations, targets and attitudes, Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT, p. 1] has characterized the vexed problem of mental representation as "the topic in the philosophy of mind for some time now." This remark is something of an understatement. The same topic was central to the famous controversy between Nicolas Malebranche and Antoine Arnauld in the 17th century and remained central to the entire philosophical tradition of "ideas" in the writings of Locke, Berkeley, Hume, Reid and Kant. However, the scholarly, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Nonconscious motor images.Giacomo Rizzolatti - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):220-220.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Call it what it is: Motor memory.Joaquin M. Fuster - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):208-208.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Motor representations and reality.M. Jeannerod - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):229-245.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Action and attention.A. H. C. Van der Heijden & Bruce Bridgeman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):225-226.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • On the relation between motor imagery and visual imagery.Roberta L. Klatzky - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):212-213.
    Jeannerod's target article describes support, through empirical and neurological findings, for the intriguing idea of motor imagery, a form of representation hypothesized to have levels of functional equivalence with motor preparation, while being consciously accessible. Jeannerod suggests that the subjectively accessible content of motor imagery allows it to be distinguished from motor preparation, which is unconscious. Motor imagery is distinguished from visual imagery in terms of content. Motor images are kinesthetic in nature; they are parametrized by variables such as force (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Moving beyond imagination.Robert Dufour, Martin H. Fischer & David A. Rosenbaum - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):206-207.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Memory organization of action events and its relationship to memory performance.Asher Koriat & Shiri Pearlman-Avnion - 2003 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 132 (3):435.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Are motor images based on kinestheticvisual matching?Robert W. Mitchell - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):214-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Motor images are action plans.Wolfgang Prinz - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):218-218.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Computation without representation.Stephen P. Stich - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):152-152.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Cognitive and motor implications of mental imagery.Romeo Chua & Daniel J. Weeks - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):203-204.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Visual-spatial movement goals.Digby Elliott & Brian K. V. Maraj - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):207-207.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Motor memory – a memory of the future.David H. Ingvar - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):210-211.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Mental representation: What language is brainese?Kim Sterelny - 1983 - Philosophical Studies 43 (May):365-82.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Separability of reference frame distinctions from motor and visual images.Gary W. Strong - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):224-225.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Structure and Strategy in Image Generation.Martha J. Farah & Stephen M. Kosslyn - 1981 - Cognitive Science 5 (4):371-383.
    Two experiments were conducted to test a prediction of the Kosslyn & Shwartz computer simulation model of mental image processing. According to this model, more complex images require more time to form because parts are placed sequentially, and larger images require more time to form than smaller ones because more parts are placed. If these accounts are correct, then the advantage of forming a small image (i.e., one that seems to subtend a smaller visual angle) should be greater for more (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Eidetic imagery, occipital EEG activity, and palinopsia.Alan Richardson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):613-613.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Motor simulation.Adam Morton - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):215-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Resemblance and imaginal representation.Ned Block - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):142-143.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Images, labels, concepts, and propositions: Some reservations regarding Premack's “abstract code”.Arthur C. Danto - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):143-144.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Language training versus training in relations.Lyn Haber - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):146-147.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Involvement of primary motor cortex in motor imagery and mental practice.Mark Hallett, Jordan Fieldman, Leonardo G. Cohen, Norihiro Sadato & Alvaro Pascual-Leone - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):210-210.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • What’s in a Mind?Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 1987 - Synthese 70 (January):97-122.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Eidetics: redefinition of the ghost and its clinical application.Akhter Ahsen - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):594-596.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark