Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. What connectionist models learn: Learning and representation in connectionist networks.Stephen José Hanson & David J. Burr - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):471-489.
    Connectionist models provide a promising alternative to the traditional computational approach that has for several decades dominated cognitive science and artificial intelligence, although the nature of connectionist models and their relation to symbol processing remains controversial. Connectionist models can be characterized by three general computational features: distinct layers of interconnected units, recursive rules for updating the strengths of the connections during learning, and “simple” homogeneous computing elements. Using just these three features one can construct surprisingly elegant and powerful models of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   62 citations  
  • Learning and representation: Tensions at the interface.Steven José Hanson - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):511-518.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Images, memory, and perception.Alastair Hannay - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):552-553.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Artificial Antisemitism: Critical Theory in the Age of Datafication.Matthew Handelman - 2022 - Critical Inquiry 48 (2):286-312.
    This article is a critical genealogy of Tay, an artificial-intelligence chatbot that Microsoft released on Twitter in 2016, which was quickly hijacked by internet trolls to reproduce racist, misogynist, and antisemitic language. Tay’s repetition and production of hate speech calls for an approach that draws on both media and cultural theory—the Frankfurt School’s dialectical analyses of language and ideology, in particular. Revisiting the Frankfurt School in the age of algorithmic reason shows that, contrary to views foundational to computing, a neural-network (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Expose hidden assumptions in network theory.Karl Haberlandt - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):495-496.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Can the aims of neuroethology be selective, while avoiding exclusivity?D. M. Guthrie - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):390-391.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Direct Associations or Internal Transformations? Exploring the Mechanisms Underlying Sequential Learning Behavior.Todd M. Gureckis & Bradley C. Love - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (1):10-50.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Stable self-organization of sensory recognition codes: Is chaos necessary?Stephen Grossberg - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):179-180.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neuroethology and theoretical neurobiology.Stephen Grossberg - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):388-390.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Brain metaphors, theories, and facts.Stephen Grossberg - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):97-98.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can mammalian pattern generators be understood?Sten Grillner - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):549-550.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Exploring Minds: Modes of Modelling and Simulation in Artificial Intelligence.Hajo Greif - 2021 - Perspectives on Science 29 (4):409-435.
    -/- The aim of this paper is to grasp the relevant distinctions between various ways in which models and simulations in Artificial Intelligence (AI) relate to cognitive phenomena. In order to get a systematic picture, a taxonomy is developed that is based on the coordinates of formal versus material analogies and theory-guided versus pre-theoretic models in science. These distinctions have parallels in the computational versus mimetic aspects and in analytic versus exploratory types of computer simulation. The proposed taxonomy cuts across (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Some recent developments on Shannon's General Purpose Analog Computer.Daniel Silva Graça - 2004 - Mathematical Logic Quarterly 50 (4-5):473-485.
    This paper revisits one of the first models of analog computation, the General Purpose Analog Computer (GPAC). In particular, we restrict our attention to the improved model presented in [11] and we show that it can be further refined. With this we prove the following: (i) the previous model can be simplified; (ii) it admits extensions having close connections with the class of smooth continuous time dynamical systems. As a consequence, we conclude that some of these extensions achieve Turing universality. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The failure of current strategies in the study of central pattern generators.Louis J. Goldberg - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):548-549.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive science and hathayoga.Ellen Goldberg - 2005 - Zygon 40 (3):613-630.
    Cognitive science and hathayoga both make emphatic claims about the relationship between the body and the mind. To examine this complementary relationship I draw upon the five main approaches currently being used by cognitive science and then consider their implications within the context of three specific points of contact with hathayoga theory: the rejection of dualism, the nature of consciousness, and the role of the nervous and circulatory systems in religious experience. This type of comparative analysis can provide additional information (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ambiguities in “the algorithmic level”.Alvin I. Goldman - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):484-485.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are connectionist models just statistical pattern classifiers?Richard M. Golden - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):494-495.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Biological foundations of the psychoneural identity hypothesis.Gordon G. Globus - 1972 - Philosophy of Science 39 (3):291-301.
    Biological foundations of the psychoneural identity hypothesis are explicated and their implications discussed. "Consciousness per se" and phenomenal contents of consciousness per se are seen to be identical with events in the (unobserved) brain in accordance with Leibniz's Law, but only informationally equivalent to neural events as observed. Phenomenal content potentially is recoverable by empirical means from observed neural events, but the converse is not possible. Consciousness per se is identical with events which do not represent anything distal to sensory (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The study of cognition and instructional design: Mutual nurturance.Robert Glaser - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):483-484.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cortical architectures and value unit encoding.Charles D. Gilbert - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):96-97.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Adaptive significance, redundancy, and variance in central pattern generators.Rhanor Gillette - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):547-548.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Central pattern generators can be understood.Peter A. Getting - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):547-547.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Models and multineuron recordings.George L. Gerstein - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):546-547.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • N-Valued Logics and Łukasiewicz–Moisil Algebras.George Georgescu - 2006 - Axiomathes 16 (1-2):123-136.
    Fundamental properties of N-valued logics are compared and eleven theorems are presented for their Logic Algebras, including Łukasiewicz–Moisil Logic Algebras represented in terms of categories and functors. For example, the Fundamental Logic Adjunction Theorem allows one to transfer certain universal, or global, properties of the Category of Boolean Algebras,, (which are well-understood) to the more general category \documentclass[12pt]{minimal} \usepackage{amsmath} \usepackage{wasysym} \usepackage{amsfonts} \usepackage{amssymb} \usepackage{amsbsy} \usepackage{mathrsfs} \usepackage{upgreek} \setlength{\oddsidemargin}{-69pt} \begin{document}$${\cal L}$$\end{document}Mn of Łukasiewicz–Moisil Algebras. Furthermore, the relationships of LMn-algebras to other many-valued logical structures, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • The virtues of chaos.Alan Garfinkel - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):178-179.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Models, modelling, and the leech swimming rhythm.W. Otto Friesen - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):546-546.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The indeterminacy of computation.Nir Fresco, B. Jack Copeland & Marty J. Wolf - 2021 - Synthese 199 (5-6):12753-12775.
    Do the dynamics of a physical system determine what function the system computes? Except in special cases, the answer is no: it is often indeterminate what function a given physical system computes. Accordingly, care should be taken when the question ‘What does a particular neuronal system do?’ is answered by hypothesising that the system computes a particular function. The phenomenon of the indeterminacy of computation has important implications for the development of computational explanations of biological systems. Additionally, the phenomenon lends (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • The Explanatory Role of Computation in Cognitive Science.Nir Fresco - 2012 - Minds and Machines 22 (4):353-380.
    Which notion of computation (if any) is essential for explaining cognition? Five answers to this question are discussed in the paper. (1) The classicist answer: symbolic (digital) computation is required for explaining cognition; (2) The broad digital computationalist answer: digital computation broadly construed is required for explaining cognition; (3) The connectionist answer: sub-symbolic computation is required for explaining cognition; (4) The computational neuroscientist answer: neural computation (that, strictly, is neither digital nor analogue) is required for explaining cognition; (5) The extreme (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Neural system stability.Walter J. Freeman - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):298-299.
    Two hypotheses concerning nonlinear elements in complex systems are contrasted: that neurons, intrinsically unstable, are stabilized through embedding in networks and populations; and, conversely, that cortical neurons are intrinsically stable, but are destabilized through embedding in cortical populations and corticostriatal feedback systems. Tests are made by piecewise linearization of nonlinear dynamics at nonequilibriumoperating points, followed by linear stability analysis.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Abstract solutions versus neurobiologically plausible problems.Jeffrey Foss - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):95-96.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What's the connection?Leif H. Finkel & George N. Reeke - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):94-95.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Using AI Methods to Evaluate a Minimal Model for Perception.Chris Fields & Robert Prentner - 2019 - Open Philosophy 2 (1):503-524.
    The relationship between philosophy and research on artificial intelligence (AI) has been difficult since its beginning, with mutual misunderstanding and sometimes even hostility. By contrast, we show how an approach informed by both philosophy and AI can be productive. After reviewing some popular frameworks for computation and learning, we apply the AI methodology of “build it and see” to tackle the philosophical and psychological problem of characterizing perception as distinct from sensation. Our model comprises a network of very simple, but (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Neuroethology according to Hoyle.Russell D. Fernald - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):387-388.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Bursting networks.John C. Fentress - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):545-546.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • So many models – So little time.Jerome A. Feldman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):551-552.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Beyond Human: Deep Learning, Explainability and Representation.M. Beatrice Fazi - forthcoming - Theory, Culture and Society:026327642096638.
    This article addresses computational procedures that are no longer constrained by human modes of representation and considers how these procedures could be philosophically understood in terms of ‘algorithmic thought’. Research in deep learning is its case study. This artificial intelligence technique operates in computational ways that are often opaque. Such a black-box character demands rethinking the abstractive operations of deep learning. The article does so by entering debates about explainability in AI and assessing how technoscience and technoculture tackle the possibility (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • On the Contribution of Neuroethics to the Ethics and Regulation of Artificial Intelligence.Michele Farisco, Kathinka Evers & Arleen Salles - 2022 - Neuroethics 15 (1):1-12.
    Contemporary ethical analysis of Artificial Intelligence is growing rapidly. One of its most recognizable outcomes is the publication of a number of ethics guidelines that, intended to guide governmental policy, address issues raised by AI design, development, and implementation and generally present a set of recommendations. Here we propose two things: first, regarding content, since some of the applied issues raised by AI are related to fundamental questions about topics like intelligence, consciousness, and the ontological and ethical status of humans, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The evolutionary aspect of cognitive functions.J. -P. Ewert - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):481-483.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Hoyle's new view of neuroethology: Limited and restrictive.J. P. Ewert - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):386-387.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Multiscale modeling of the brain should be validated in more detail against the biological data.Harry R. Erwin - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):297-298.
    Wright & Liley provide an advance in addressing the interaction of multiple scales of processing in the brain. It should address in more detail the biological evidence that underlies the models it proposes to replace.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The scientific induction problem: A case for case studies.K. Anders Ericsson - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):480-481.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neuroethology or motorethology?Joachim Erber - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):386-386.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How we ought to describe computation in the brain.Chris Eliasmith - 2010 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 41 (3):313-320.
    I argue that of the four kinds of quantitative description relevant for understanding brain function, a control theoretic approach is most appealing. This argument proceeds by comparing computational, dynamical, statistical and control theoretic approaches, and identifying criteria for a good description of brain function. These criteria include providing useful decompositions, simple state mappings, and the ability to account for variability. The criteria are justified by their importance in providing unified accounts of multi-level mechanisms that support intervention. Evaluation of the four (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Some agenda items for a neurobiology of cognition: An introduction.Peter D. Eimas & Albert M. Galaburda - 1989 - Cognition 33 (1-2):1-23.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Disregarding vertebrates is neither useful nor necessary.Günter Ehret - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):385-386.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the differences between cognitive and noncognitive systems.D. C. Earle - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):177-178.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Investigation of the dynamics of the some class of neuronet represented by weeknonlinear difference systems.Khusainov D. Y., Shatyrko A. V., Puzha B., Novotna V. & Pylypenko V. A. - 2019 - Artificial Intelligence Scientific Journal 24 (1-2):49-58.
    The article is devoted to dynamic processes in the field of artificial intelligence, namely in the tasks of neurodynamics. The problems of stability of transient processes in neural networks, which dynamics can be described by systems of weakly nonlinear difference equations, are considered. Conditions are formulated in terms of the direct Lyapunov method.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The experience dependent dynamics of human consciousness.Birgitta Dresp-Langley - 2018 - Open Journal of Philosophy 8 (2):116-143.
    By reviewing most of the neurobiology of consciousness, this article highlights some major reasons why a successful emulation of the dynamics of human consciousness by artificial intelligence is unlikely. The analysis provided leads to conclude that human consciousness is epigenetically determined and experience and context-dependent at the individual level. It is subject to changes in time that are essentially unpredictable. If cracking the code to human consciousness were possible, the result would most likely have to consist of a temporal pattern (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Hermann Schmidt (1894–1968) et la théorie générale de la régulation: Une cybernétique allemande en 1940?Frank Dittmann & Jérôme Ségal - 1997 - Annals of Science 54 (6):547-565.
    La théorie générale du contrôle et de la communication, mieux connue sous le nom de ‘cybernétique’, est habituellement considérée comme le fruit des recherches liées à la deuxième guerre mondiale aux Etats-Unis, menées principalement par le mathématicien américain Norbert Wiener . Dans le cadre de son travail pour l'armée, Wiener a développé une théorie générale des régulations. Nous nous intéressons aux origines de la théorie générale des régulations en Allemagne , montrant qu'elle prend naissance dans un contexte bien différent.Hermann Schmidt (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On interpretative processes in imagery.Manuel de Vega - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):551-551.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark