Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory.Nash Unsworth & Randall W. Engle - 2007 - Psychological Review 114 (1):104-132.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   69 citations  
  • Serial learning and filled and unfilled delay intervals: Effects of informative feedback contingencies.Sam S. Rakover & Malka Maon - 1973 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 2 (2):87-88.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The effects of instructions, evaluative feedback, and knowledge of results upon the short-term retention of ninth graders.Kenneth L. Witte & James Huntermark - 1975 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 5 (1):79-81.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Delayed matching-to-sample performance as a measure of human visuospatial working memory.Wendy V. Parr - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (5):369-372.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Short-term recognition memory under rehearsal instructions and imaging instructions.C. Alan Boneau - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (4):297-299.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Lexical learning in bilingual adults: The relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and phonological knowledge.Steve Majerus, Martine Poncelet, Martial Van der Linden & Brendan S. Weekes - 2008 - Cognition 107 (2):395-419.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Mapping speech: More analysis, less synthesis, please.Michael Studdert-Kennedy - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (2):218-219.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Brain organization for language from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping.George A. Ojemann - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (2):189-206.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   129 citations  
  • What do reinforcers strengthen? The unit of selection.John W. Donahoe - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):138-139.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Memories and functional response units.Kennon A. Lattal & Josele Abreu-Rodrigues - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):143-144.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From overt behavior to hypothetical behavior to memory: Inference in the wrong direction.Howard Rachlin - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):147-148.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   66 citations  
  • Awareness and reinforcement.Charles P. Shimp - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):149-150.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The phenomenal object of memory and control processes.Giuliana Mazzoni - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):202-203.
    This commentary deals with criteria for assigning truth values to memory contents. A parallel with perception shows how truth values can be assigned by considering subjects' beliefs about the truth state of the memory content. This topic is also relevant to the study of processes of control over retrieval.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Metacognition, metaphors, and the measurement of human memory.Thomas O. Nelson - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):204-205.
    Investigations of metacognition – and also the application of the storehouse and correspondence metaphors – seem as appropriate for laboratory research as for naturalistic research. In terms of measurement, the only quantitative difference between the “input-bound percent correct” and “output-bound percent correct” is the inclusion versus exclusion (respectively) of omission errors in the denominator of the percentages.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The alternative to the storehouse metaphor.Aaron Ben-Ze'ev - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):192-193.
    Koriat and Goldsmith clearly show the need for an alternative to the storehouse metaphor; however, the alternative metaphor they choose – the correspondence metaphor – is problematic. A more suitable one is the capacity metaphor.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Implications of output-bound measures for laboratory and field research in memory.Ronald P. Fisher - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):197-197.
    Everyday memory tasks often require that researchers focus on output-bound measures of memory. As a result, nonmemorial processes (e.g., report option and grain size) may influence recall accuracy. These nonmemorial processes, usually eliminated by laboratory researchers, have the potential to explain some anomalous results and may even be useful to enhance everyday recollection.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A gap in Nisbett and Wilson’s findings? A first-person access to our cognitive processes.Claire Petitmengin, Anne Remillieux, Béatrice Cahour & Shirley Carter-Thomas - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (2):654-669.
    The well-known experiments of Nisbett and Wilson lead to the conclusion that we have no introspective access to our decision-making processes. Johansson et al. have recently developed an original protocol consisting in manipulating covertly the relationship between the subjects’ intended choice and the outcome they were presented with: in 79.6% of cases, they do not detect the manipulation and provide an explanation of the choice they did not make, confirming the findings of Nisbett and Wilson. We have reproduced this protocol, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • An empirical study on using visual metaphors in visualization.Rita Borgo, Alfie Abdul-Rahman, Mohamed Farhan, Philip W. Grant, Irene Reppa, Luciano Floridi & Min Chen - 2012 - IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics 18 (12):2759-2768.
    In written and spoken communications, metaphors are often used as an aid to help convey abstract or less tangible concepts. However, the benefits of using visual metaphors in visualization have so far been inconclusive. In this work, we report an empirical study to evaluate hypotheses that visual metaphors may aid memorization, visual search and concept comprehension. One major departure from previous metaphor-related experiments in the literature is that we make use of a dual-task methodology in our experiment. This design offers (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neocortical memory traces.Earl K. Miller - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):488-489.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Identification and Evaluation of Neuropsychological Tools Used in the Assessment of Alcohol-Related Cognitive Impairment: A Systematic Review.Robert Heirene, Bev John & Gareth Roderique-Davies - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The development of memory maintenance strategies: training cumulative rehearsal and interactive imagery in children aged between 5 and 9.Sadie Miller, Samantha McCulloch & Christopher Jarrold - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:128596.
    The current study explored the extent to which children above and below the age of 7 years are able to benefit from either training in cumulative rehearsal or in the use of interactive imagery when carrying out working memory tasks. Twenty-four 5- to 6-year-olds and 24 8- to 9-year olds were each assigned to one of three training groups who either received cumulative rehearsal, interactive imagery, or passive labelling training. Participants’ ability to maintain material during a filled delay was then (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “Do we need to memorize that?” or cognitive science for chemists.JudithAnn R. Hartman & Eric A. Nelson - 2015 - Foundations of Chemistry 17 (3):263-274.
    In introductory chemistry courses, should students be encouraged to solve problems by reasoning based on conceptual understanding or by applying memorized facts and algorithms? Cognitive scientists have recently studied this issue with the assistance of new technologies. In the current consensus model for cognition, during problem solving the brain relies on “working memory” to sequentially process small elements of knowledge. Working memory is able to hold and manipulate virtually all elements that can be recalled “with automaticity” from long-term memory, but (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Hippocampal modulation of recognition, conditioning, timing, and space: Why so many functions?Stephen Grossberg - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):479-480.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Functional components of the hippocampal memory system: Implications for future learning and memory research in nonhuman primates.Peter R. Rapp - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):491-492.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The effect of instructions to forget on proactive inhibition.Maria Regina Coccia & Delos D. Wickens - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 7 (5):479-480.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Delayed matching-to-sample in rats in a Y-maze: Instances of facilitation and immediate cross-modal transfer.M. Ray Denny, Carla Clos & Mark Rilling - 1989 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 27 (2):141-144.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system.Howard Eichenbaum, Tim Otto & Neal J. Cohen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):449-472.
    There is considerable evidence that the hippocampal system contributes both to (1) the temporary maintenance of memories and to (2) the processing of a particular type of memory representation. The findings on amnesia suggest that these two distinguishing features of hippocampal memory processing are orthogonal. Together with anatomical and physiological data, the neuropsychological findings support a model of cortico-hippocampal interactions in which the temporal and representational properties of hippocampal memory processing are mediated separately. We propose that neocortical association areas maintain (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   143 citations  
  • Motor theory of speech perception or acoustic theory of speech production?Lyn Frazier - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (2):213-214.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Remembering as doing.Ulric Neisser - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):203-204.
    Koriat & Goldsmith are right in their claim that the “ecological” and “traditional” approaches to memory rely on different metaphors. But the underlying ecological metaphor is notcorrespondence(which in any case is not a metaphorical notion): it isaction. Remembering is a kind of doing; like most other forms of action it is purposive, personal, and particular.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Operationaling “correspondence”.David C. Palmer - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):206-207.
    The research guided by the correspondence metaphor is lauded for its emphasis on functional analysis, but the term “correspondence” itself needs clarification. Of the two terms in the relationship, only one is well defined. It is suggested that behavior at acquisition needs to be analyzed and that molecular principles from the learning laboratory might be useful in doing so.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On correspondence, accuracy, and truth.Ian Maynard Begg - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):191-192.
    Koriat & Goldsmith raise important questions about memory, but there is need for caution: first, if we define accuracy by output measures, there is a danger that a perfectly accurate memory can be nearly useless. Second, when we focus on correspondence, there is a danger that syntactic correspondence will be mistaken for historical truth.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Memory, metamemory, and conditional statistics.Robert A. Bjork & Thomas D. Wickens - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):193-194.
    Koriat & Goldsmith's distinction between encoding processes and metamnemonic decision processes is theoretically and practically important, as is their methodology for separating the two. However, their accuracy measure is a conditional statistic, subject to the unfathomable selection effects that have hindered analogous measures in the past. We also find their arguments concerning basic and applied research mostly beside the point.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Correspondence conception of memory: A good match is hard to find.Daniel Algom - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):188-189.
    The distinction that Koriat & Goldsmith have drawn between laboratory and naturalistic research is largely valid, but the metaphor they have chosen to characterize the latter may not be optimal. The “correspondence” approach is vulnerable on conceptual grounds and is not applicable to significant portions of empirical research.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mechanisms in cognitive psychology: What are the operations?William Bechtel - 2008 - Philosophy of Science 75 (5):983-994.
    Cognitive psychologists, like biologists, frequently describe mechanisms when explaining phenomena. Unlike biologists, who can often trace material transformations to identify operations, psychologists face a more daunting task in identifying operations that transform information. Behavior provides little guidance as to the nature of the operations involved. While not itself revealing the operations, identification of brain areas involved in psychological mechanisms can help constrain attempts to characterize the operations. In current memory research, evidence that the same brain areas are involved in what (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Hippocampal neuronal activity in rat and primate: Memory and movement.Frasar A. W. Wilson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):499-500.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • When Learning Disturbs Memory – Temporal Profile of Retroactive Interference of Learning on Memory Formation.Zrinka Sosic-Vasic, Katrin Hille, Julia Kröner, Manfred Spitzer & Jürgen Kornmeier - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Demise of Short-Term Memory Revisited: Empirical and Computational Investigations of Recency Effects.Eddy J. Davelaar, Yonatan Goshen-Gottstein, Amir Ashkenazi, Henk J. Haarmann & Marius Usher - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (1):3-42.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Hippocampus, space, and relations.Lynn Nadel - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):490-491.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Does it still make sense to develop a declarative memory theory of hippocampal function?J. N. P. Rawlins, R. M. J. Deacon, B. K. Yee & H. J. Cassaday - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):492-493.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure?Sian L. Beilock & Thomas H. Carr - 2001 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130 (4):701.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   97 citations  
  • Functional analysis and mechanistic explanation.David Barrett - 2014 - Synthese 191 (12):2695-2714.
    Piccinini and Craver (Synthese 183:283–311, 2011) argue for the surprising view that psychological explanation, properly understood, is a species of mechanistic explanation. This contrasts with the ‘received view’ (due, primarily, to Cummins and Fodor) which maintains a sharp distinction between psychological explanation and mechanistic explanation. The former is typically construed as functional analysis, the analysis of some psychological capacity into an organized series of subcapacities without specifying any of the structural features that underlie the explanandum capacity. The latter idea, of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • The secondary memory component in the Brown-Peterson paradigm.D. J. Murray - 1979 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 13 (2):64-66.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A step linking memory to understanding?Mark A. Good & Richard G. M. Morris - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):477-478.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Timing sequencers as a foundation for language.William H. Calvin - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (2):210-211.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Contexts and functions of retrieval.Eugene Winograd - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):209-210.
    Koriat & Goldsmith provide an excellent analysis of the flexibility of retrieval processes and how they are situationally dependent. I agree with their emphasis on functional considerations and argue that the traditional laboratory experiment motivates the subject to be accurate. However, I disagree with their strong claim that the quantity–accuracy distinction implies an essential discontinuity between traditional and naturalistic approaches to the study of memory.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Classical antecedents for modern metaphors for memory.Jocelyn Penny Small - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):208-208.
    Classical antiquity provides not just the storehouse metaphor, which postdates Plato, but also parts of the correspondence metaphor. In the fifth century B.C., Thucydides (1.22) considered the role of gist and accuracy in writing history, and Aristotle (Poetics1451b, 1460b 8–11) offered an explanation. Finally, the Greek for truth (alêtheia) means “that which is not forgotten.”.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Let's forget the everyday/laboratory controversy.Lia Kvavilashvili & Judi Ellis - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):199-200.
    In contrast to its aims, Koriat & Goldsmith's article vividly demonstrates(1) the complementarity of ecological and traditional approaches and (2) the difficulty of characterising the growing diversity of memory research with a single set of distinctions. Moreover, the contrast between correspondence and storehouse metaphors is important enough to stand alone without reference to an everyday/laboratory controversy, which is neither acute nor necessary.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Correspondence to the past: The essence of the archaeology metaphor.Steen F. Larsen - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):200-201.
    The correspondence view of memory is not a metaphor. However, correspondence is the essential feature of the archaeology metaphor, which harks back to Freud and Neisser. A modern version of this metaphor and some of its implications are briefly described. The archaeology metaphor integrates the idea of stored traces in a nonmechanistic framework.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Accuracy and quantity are poor measures of recall and recognition.Andrew R. Mayes, Rob van Eijk & Patricia L. Gooding - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):201-202.
    The value of accuracy and quantity as memory measures is assessed. It is argued that (1) accuracy does not measure correspondence (monitoring) because it ignores omissions and correct rejections, (2) quantity is confounded with monitoring in recall, and (3) in recognition, if targets and foils are unequal, both measures, even together, still ignore correct rejections.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • False dichotomies and dead metaphors.Timothy P. McNamara - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):203-203.
    Koriat & Goldsmith's thesis is provocative but has three problems: First, quantity and accuracy are not simply related, they are complementary. Second, the storehouse metaphor is not the driving force behind contemporary theories of memory and may not be viable. Third, the taxonomy is incomplete, leaving unclassified several extremely influential methods and measures, such as priming and response latency.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark