Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Ludwik Flecks Gestaltbegriff und sein Blick auf die Gestaltpsychologie seiner Zeit.Claus Zittel - 2014 - NTM Zeitschrift für Geschichte der Wissenschaften, Technik und Medizin 22 (1-2):9-29.
    The notion of ‘Gestalt’ plays a prominent role in Ludwik Fleck’s theory of thought styles. This paper scrutinizes how Fleck adopted the concepts and even methods of Gestalt psychology that he sometimes vaguely refers to. Systematically comparing the argumentation and theoretical outlines of Fleck’s social theory of perception and the principles of some Gestalt theories, this article will show and discuss their similarities and fundamental differences. According to Fleck, both science and individual perception rest on social actions and cultural traditions. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Topological Foundations of Cognitive Science.Carola Eschenbach, Christopher Habel & Barry Smith (eds.) - 1984 - Hamburg: Graduiertenkolleg Kognitionswissenschaft.
    A collection of papers presented at the First International Summer Institute in Cognitive Science, University at Buffalo, July 1994, including the following papers: ** Topological Foundations of Cognitive Science, Barry Smith ** The Bounds of Axiomatisation, Graham White ** Rethinking Boundaries, Wojciech Zelaniec ** Sheaf Mereology and Space Cognition, Jean Petitot ** A Mereotopological Definition of 'Point', Carola Eschenbach ** Discreteness, Finiteness, and the Structure of Topological Spaces, Christopher Habel ** Mass Reference and the Geometry of Solids, Almerindo E. Ojeda (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Extending Gurwitsch’s field theory of consciousness.Jeff Yoshimi & David W. Vinson - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 34:104-123.
    Aron Gurwitsch’s theory of the structure and dynamics of consciousness has much to offer contemporary theorizing about consciousness and its basis in the embodied brain. On Gurwitsch’s account, as we develop it, the field of consciousness has a variable sized focus or "theme" of attention surrounded by a structured periphery of inattentional contents. As the field evolves, its contents change their status, sometimes smoothly, sometimes abruptly. Inner thoughts, a sense of one’s body, and the physical environment are dominant field contents. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Perceptual influence of elementary three-dimensional geometry: objectness.Florentin Wörgötter, Rahel M. Sutterlütti & Minija Tamosiunaite - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Comparative cognition of spatial representation.Donald M. Wilkie & Robert J. Wilison - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):97-98.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cross-cultural research needs crossfertilisation.Peter Wenderoth - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):97-97.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Perceptions in perspective.R. A. Weale - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):96-97.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Figure–ground organization and the emergence of proto-objects in the visual cortex.Rüdiger von der Heydt - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Cross-cultural research in perception: The missing theoretical perspective.Fons J. R. van de Vijver & Ype H. Poortinga - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):95-96.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cultural determination of picture space: The acid test.E. Broydrick Thro - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):94-95.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Perceptual influence of elementary three-dimensional geometry: (2) fundamental object parts.Minija Tamosiunaite, Rahel M. Sutterlütti, Simon C. Stein & Florentin Wörgötter - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Thinking, Experiencing and Rethinking Mereological Interdependence.Michael W. Stadler - 2019 - Gestalt Theory 41 (1):31-46.
    Summary The present article is a partly ontological, partly Gestalt-psychological discussion of the thinkability of structures in which parts and whole are interdependent (MI). In the first section, I show that in the framework of E. Husserl’s formal part–whole ontology, the conceptualization of such an interdependence leads to (mereo)logical problems. The second section turns to and affirms the experience of this interplay between parts and whole, exemplified with B. Pinna’s recent research on meaningful Gestalt perception. In the final section, I (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Editorial: Color and Space in Perception and Art.Branka Spehar & Tiziano Agostini - 2022 - Gestalt Theory 44 (1-2):1-6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Whither cross-cultural perception?Daniel W. Smothergill - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):93-94.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dissociation of processing time and awareness by the inattentional blindness paradigm☆.Shih-Yu Lo & Su-Ling Yeh - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (4):1169-1180.
    Consciousness researchers are interested in distinguishing between mental activity that occurs with and without awareness . The inattentional blindness paradigm is an excellent tool for this question because it permits the independent manipulation of processing time and awareness. In the present study, we show that implicit texture segregation can occur during inattentional blindness, provided that the texture is exposed for a sufficient duration. In contrast, a Simon effect does not occur during inattentional blindness, even with similar exposure duration of the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Ernst Mach and George Sarton’s Successors: The Implicit Role Model of Teaching Science in USA and Elsewhere, Part II.Hayo Siemsen - 2013 - Science & Education 22 (5):951-1000.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Application of a Phenomenological Method To the Faces-Goblet Stimulus Display: I. Initiating the Inquiry and Defining Figure and Ground.Ronald E. Shor - 1979 - Journal of Phenomenological Psychology 10 (2):189-231.
    "I don't want to hurry it ... When you want to hurry something, that means you no longer care about it and want to get on to other things. I just want to get at it slowly, but carefully and thoroughly, with the same attitude I remember was present just before . It is that attitude that found it, nothing else.".
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Experiencing the a priori.Denis Seron - 2021 - European Journal of Philosophy 31 (2):371-379.
    Brentano clearly asserts, in his Vienna lectures of 1887–1888, that his descriptive psychology is an a priori or “exact” science. Since he rejects Kant's idea of a synthetic a priori, this means that the descriptive psychologist's laws are analytic. My aim in this paper is to clarify and discuss this view. I examine Brentano's epistemology in the Psychology from an Empirical Standpoint and then its later developments. I conclude with a difficulty inherent in Brentano's psychological approach to a priori knowledge.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Questions: An essay in Daubertian phenomenology.Karl Schuhmann & Barry Smith - 1987 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 47 (3):353-384.
    A number of logicians and philosophers have turned their attention in recent years to the problem of developing a logic of interrogatives. Their work has thrown a great deal of light on the formal properties of questions and question-sentences and has led also to interesting innovations in our understanding of the structures of performatives in general and, for example, in the theory of presuppositions. When, however, we examine the attempts of logicians such as Belnap or Åqvist to specify what, precisely, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Many a slip 'twixt external and internal representation.David Rose - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):93-93.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pictures, maybe; illusions, no.Robert H. Pollack - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):92-93.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Directional organization and shape formation: new illusions and Helmholtz's Square.Baingio Pinna - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Plea for more exploration of cross-cultural cognitive space.David Piggins - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):91-92.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Hearing what the body feels: Auditory encoding of rhythmic movement.Jessica Phillips-Silver & Laurel J. Trainor - 2007 - Cognition 105 (3):533-546.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • The archaeology of space: Real and representational.Christopher S. Peebles - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):91-91.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Written Argumentation by a 10-Year-Old Pupil in Sweden.Ewa Bergh Nestlog - 2009 - Argumentation 23 (4):437-449.
    Most pupils become confident with narrative texts. However, studies show that pupils do not learn to master discursive genres in a satisfactory way. Therefore it is important to study pupils’ written argumentation and to develop knowledge about text production in an education that also highlights linguistic structures. The present article investigates written argumentations produced by 10–12 year-old pupils. The aim is to investigate perspectives in the texts, and thereby catch the entire texts—their content, function and form—and to relate text analysis (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Phenomenological Descriptions After the Manner of Edgar Rubin.I. K. Moustgaard - 1975 - Journal of Phenomenological Psychology 6 (1):31-61.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Construal in language: A visual-world approach to the effects of linguistic alternations on event perception and conception.Srdan Medimorec, Petar Milin & Dagmar Divjak - 2020 - Cognitive Linguistics 31 (1):37-72.
    The theoretical notion of ‘construal’ captures the idea that the way in which we describe a scene reflects our conceptualization of it. Relying on the concept of ception – which conjoins conception and perception – we operationalized construal and employed a Visual World Paradigm to establish which aspects of linguistic scene description modulate visual scene perception, thereby affecting event conception. By analysing viewing behaviour after alternating ways of describing location (prepositions), agentivity (active/passive voice) and transfer (NP/pp datives), we found that (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Real space and represented space: Crosscultural convergences.Harry McGurk - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):90-91.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Gestalt isomorphism and the primacy of the subjective perceptual experience.Steven Lehar - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (6):763-764.
    The Gestalt principle of isomorphism reveals the primacy of subjective experience as a valid source of evidence for the information encoded neurophysiologically. This theory invalidates the abstractionist view that the neurophysiological representation can be of lower dimensionality than the percept to which it gives rise.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Multimodal Sensory-Spatial Integration and Retrieval of Trained Motor Patterns for Body Coordination in Musicians and Dancers.Aija Marie Ladda, Sarah B. Wallwork & Martin Lotze - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Dancers and musicians are experts in spatial and temporal processing which allows them to coordinate movement with music. This high-level processing has been associated with structural and functional adaptation of the brain for high performance sensorimotor integration. For these integration processes, adaptation does not only take place in primary and secondary sensory and motor areas but tertiary brain areas, such as the lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC) and the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), also provide vital resources for highly specialized performance. Here, we (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • An enactive and dynamical systems theory account of dyadic relationships.Miriam Kyselo & Wolfgang Tschacher - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Universals of depiction, illusion as nonpictorial, and limits to depiction.John M. Kennedy - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):88-90.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the rationale for cross-cultural research.G. Jahoda - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):87-88.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ambiguous figures and the spatial contents of perceptual experience: a defense of representationalism.René Jagnow - 2011 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 10 (3):325-346.
    Representationalists hold that the phenomenal character of a perceptual experience is identical with, or supervenes on, an aspect of its representational content. As such, representationalism could be disproved by a counter-example consisting of two experiences that have the same representational content but differ in phenomenal character. In this paper, I discuss two recently proposed counter-examples to representationalism that involve ambiguous or reversible figures. I pursue two goals. My first, and most important, goal is to show that the representationalist can offer (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Picture in visual space and recognition of similarity.Tarow Indow - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):87-87.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Different skills or different knowledge?Timothy L. Hubbard, John C. Baird & Asir Ajmal - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):86-87.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The representation of space: In the 2/3i of the beholder.Stephen C. Hirtle - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):85-85.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Invisibility and interpretation.Michael H. Herzog, Frouke Hermens & Haluk Öğmen - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Dual frames for causal induction: the normative and the heuristic.Ikuko Hattori, Masasi Hattori, David E. Over, Tatsuji Takahashi & Jean Baratgin - 2017 - Thinking and Reasoning 23 (3):292-317.
    Causal induction in the real world often has to be quick and efficient as well as accurate. We propose that people use two different frames to achieve these goals. The A-frame consists of heuristic processes that presuppose rarity and can detect causally relevant factors quickly. The B-frame consists of analytic processes that can be highly accurate in detecting actual causes. Our dual frame theory implies that several factors affect whether people use the A-frame or the B-frame in causal induction: among (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Things and pictures of things: Are perceptual processes invariant across cultures?Diane F. Halpern - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):84-85.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • When “All the Five Circles” are Four: New Exercises in Domain Restriction.Bart Geurts & Bob van Tiel - 2016 - Topoi 35 (1):109-122.
    The domain of a quantifier is determined by a variety of factors, which broadly speaking fall into two types. On the one hand, the context of utterance plays a role: if the focus of attention is on a particular collection of kangaroos, for example, then “Q kangaroos” is likely to range over the individuals in that set. On the other hand, the utterance itself will help to establish the quantificational domain, inter alia through presuppositions triggered within the sentence. In this (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • A computational approach to picture production and consumption is needed right here.Norman H. Freeman - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):82-84.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Seeing Double: Exploring the Phenomenology of Self-Reported Absence of Rivalry in Bistable Pictures.Elisa Filevich, Maxi Becker, Yuan-hao Wu & Simone Kühn - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The distinction between object recognition and picture recognition.Hadyn D. Ellis - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):81-82.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • What you see isn't always what you know.John Eliot - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):80-81.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Segmentation, attention and phenomenal visual objects.Jon Driver, Greg Davis, Charlotte Russell, Massimo Turatto & Elliot Freeman - 2001 - Cognition 80 (1-2):61-95.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  • The Kanizsa square does not engender a configural superiority effect.Birgitta Dresp - 1993 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 31 (3):183-184.
    This article presents psychophysical evidence that the Kanizsa Square does not produce an 'object superiority effect' previously reported in similar Gestalt configurations. Implications of the findings for Gestalt theory are addressed.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Principles of perceptual grouping: implications for image-guided surgery.Birgitta Dresp-Langley - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    The laws and principles which predict how perceptual qualities can be extracted from the most elementary visual signals were discovered by the Gestalt psychologists(e.g., Wertheimer,1923; Metzger,1930, translated and re-editedbySpillmann in 2009 and2012, respectively). Their seminal work has inspired visual science ever since, andhas led to exciting discoveries which have confirmed the Gestalt idea that the human brain would have an astonishing capacity for selecting and combining critical visual signals to generate output representations for decision making and action. This capacity of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Neural Computation of Surface Border Ownership and Relative Surface Depth from Ambiguous Contrast Inputs.Birgitta Dresp-Langley & Stephen Grossberg - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    The segregation of image parts into foreground and background is an important aspect of the neural computation of 3D scene perception. To achieve such segregation, the brain needs information about border ownership; that is, the belongingness of a contour to a specific surface represented in the image. This article presents psychophysical data derived from 3D percepts of figure and ground that were generated by presenting 2D images composed of spatially disjoint shapes that pointed inward or outward relative to the continuous (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations