Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The Typicality Effect in Basic Needs.Thomas Pölzler & Ivar R. Hannikainen - 2022 - Synthese 200 (5):1-26.
    According to the so-called Classical Theory, concepts are mentally represented by individually necessary and jointly sufficient application conditions. One of the principal empirical objections against this view stems from evidence that people judge some instances of a concept to be more typical than others. In this paper we present and discuss four empirical studies that investigate the extent to which this ‘typicality effect’ holds for the concept of basic needs. Through multiple operationalizations of typicality, our studies yielded evidence for a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Images, memory, and perception.Alastair Hannay - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):552-553.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Similarity-based categorization and fuzziness of natural categories.James A. Hampton - 1998 - Cognition 65 (2-3):137-165.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • A demonstration of intransitivity in natural categories.James A. Hampton - 1982 - Cognition 12 (2):151-164.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Three frames suffice: Drop the retinotopic frame.Ralph Norman Haber - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):295-296.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Graph‐Theoretic Properties of Networks Based on Word Association Norms: Implications for Models of Lexical Semantic Memory.Thomas M. Gruenenfelder, Gabriel Recchia, Tim Rubin & Michael N. Jones - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (6):1460-1495.
    We compared the ability of three different contextual models of lexical semantic memory and of a simple associative model to predict the properties of semantic networks derived from word association norms. None of the semantic models were able to accurately predict all of the network properties. All three contextual models over-predicted clustering in the norms, whereas the associative model under-predicted clustering. Only a hybrid model that assumed that some of the responses were based on a contextual model and others on (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Four frames do not suffice.Stephen Grossberg - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):294-295.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • The empirical case for role-governed categories.Micah B. Goldwater, Arthur B. Markman & C. Hunt Stilwell - 2011 - Cognition 118 (3):359-376.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Picture naming.Wilhelm R. Glaser - 1992 - Cognition 42 (1-3):61-105.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Linking features in dimensions of mind and brain.Robert B. Glassman - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):293-294.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Alternate conceptions of semantic memory.Arnold L. Glass & Keith J. Holyoak - 1974 - Cognition 3 (4):313-339.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • On the notion of concept I.Michael Freund - 2008 - Artificial Intelligence 172 (4-5):570-590.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • On Categorial Membership.Michael Freund - 2014 - Erkenntnis 79 (5):1045-1068.
    We investigate the family of concepts that an agent comes to know through a set of defining features, and examine the role played by these features in the process of categorization. In a qualitative framework, categorial membership is evaluated through an order relation among the objects at hand, which translates the fact that an object may fall more than another under a given concept. For concepts defined by their features, this global membership order depends on the degree with which each (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Incremental Bayesian Category Learning From Natural Language.Lea Frermann & Mirella Lapata - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (6):1333-1381.
    Models of category learning have been extensively studied in cognitive science and primarily tested on perceptual abstractions or artificial stimuli. In this paper, we focus on categories acquired from natural language stimuli, that is, words. We present a Bayesian model that, unlike previous work, learns both categories and their features in a single process. We model category induction as two interrelated subproblems: the acquisition of features that discriminate among categories, and the grouping of concepts into categories based on those features. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Tunnel vision will not suffice.Jerome A. Feldman - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):302-313.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • So many models – So little time.Jerome A. Feldman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):551-552.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Four frames suffice: A provisional model of vision and space.Jerome A. Feldman - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):265-289.
    This paper presents a general computational treatment of how mammals are able to deal with visual objects and environments. The model tries to cover the entire range from behavior and phenomenological experience to detailed neural encodings in crude but computationally plausible reductive steps. The problems addressed include perceptual constancies, eye movements and the stable visual world, object descriptions, perceptual generalizations, and the representation of extrapersonal space.The entire development is based on an action-oriented notion of perception. The observer is assumed to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   207 citations  
  • Connectionist Models and Their Properties.J. A. Feldman & D. H. Ballard - 1982 - Cognitive Science 6 (3):205-254.
    Much of the progress in the fields constituting cognitive science has been based upon the use of explicit information processing models, almost exclusively patterned after conventional serial computers. An extension of these ideas to massively parallel, connectionist models appears to offer a number of advantages. After a preliminary discussion, this paper introduces a general connectionist model and considers how it might be used in cognitive science. Among the issues addressed are: stability and noise‐sensitivity, distributed decision‐making, time and sequence problems, and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   442 citations  
  • There is more going on in the human mind.Géry D'Ydewalle & Rudi Peeters - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):239.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Theories of perception as experimental epistemology.P. C. Dodwell - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):291-293.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Items Outperform Adjectives in a Computational Model of Binary Semantic Classification.Evgeniia Diachek, Sarah Brown-Schmidt & Sean M. Polyn - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (9):e13336.
    Semantic memory encompasses one's knowledge about the world. Distributional semantic models, which construct vector spaces with embedded words, are a proposed framework for understanding the representational structure of human semantic knowledge. Unlike some classic semantic models, distributional semantic models lack a mechanism for specifying the properties of concepts, which raises questions regarding their utility for a general theory of semantic knowledge. Here, we develop a computational model of a binary semantic classification task, in which participants judged target words for the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On interpretative processes in imagery.Manuel de Vega - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):551-551.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A Memory‐Based Theory of Verbal Cognition.Simon Dennis - 2005 - Cognitive Science 29 (2):145-193.
    The syntagmatic paradigmatic model is a distributed, memory‐based account of verbal processing. Built on a Bayesian interpretation of string edit theory, it characterizes the control of verbal cognition as the retrieval of sets of syntagmatic and paradigmatic constraints from sequential and relational long‐term memory and the resolution of these constraints in working memory. Lexical information is extracted directly from text using a version of the expectation maximization algorithm. In this article, the model is described and then illustrated on a number (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • An Attractor Model of Lexical Conceptual Processing: Simulating Semantic Priming.George S. Cree, Ken McRae & Chris McNorgan - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (3):371-414.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • Modeling the mind's eye.Lynn A. Cooper - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):550-551.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A Taxonomy of Part‐Whole Relations.Roger Chaffin - 1987 - Cognitive Science 11 (4):417-444.
    A taxonomy of part‐whole or meronymic relations is developed to explain the ordinary English‐speaker's use of the term “part of” and its cognates. The resulting classification yields six types of meronymic relations: 1. component‐integral object (pedal‐bike), 2. member‐collection (ship‐fleet), 3. portion‐mass (slice‐pie), 4. stuff‐object (steel‐car), 5. feature‐activity (paying‐shopping), and 6. place‐area (Everglades‐Florida). Meronymic relations ore further distinguished from other inclusion relations, such as spatial inclusion, and class inclusion, and from several other semantic relations: attribution, attachment, and ownership. This taxonomy is (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Quantifying the Interplay of Semantics and Phonology During Failures of Word Retrieval by People With Aphasia Using a Multiplex Lexical Network.Nichol Castro, Massimo Stella & Cynthia S. Q. Siew - 2020 - Cognitive Science 44 (9):e12881.
    Investigating instances where lexical selection fails can lead to deeper insights into the cognitive machinery and architecture supporting successful word retrieval and speech production. In this paper, we used a multiplex lexical network approach that combines semantic and phonological similarities among words to model the structure of the mental lexicon. Network measures at different levels of analysis (degree, network distance, and closeness centrality) were used to investigate the influence of network structure on picture naming accuracy and errors by people with (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Could three frames suffice?Roger A. Browse & Brian E. Butler - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):290-291.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • Neurologizing mental imagery: the physiological optics of the mind's eye.Bruce Bridgeman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):550-550.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Similarity Reimagined (with Implications for a Theory of Concepts).Corinne L. Bloch-Mullins - 2021 - Theoria 87 (1):31-68.
    Similarity‐based theories of concepts have a broad intuitive appeal and have been successful in accounting for various phenomena related to the formation and application of concepts. Their adequacy as theories of concepts has been questioned, however, as similarity is often taken as too flexible, too unconstrained, to be explanatory of categorization. In this article, I propose an account of similarity that takes the “foil” against which the target items are measured as integral to the process of comparison, making the similarity (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Bridging the Gap between Similarity and Causality: An Integrated Approach to Concepts.Corinne L. Bloch-Mullins - 2018 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 69 (3):605-632.
    A growing consensus in the philosophy and psychology of concepts is that while theories such as the prototype, exemplar, and theory theories successfully account for some instances of concept formation and application, none of them successfully accounts for all such instances. I argue against this ‘new consensus’ and show that the problem is, in fact, more severe: the explanatory force of each of these theories is limited even with respect to the phenomena often cited to support it, as each fails (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Neuropsychological evidence and the semantic/episodic distinction.Alan D. Baddeley - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):238.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The Folk Psychology of Consciousness.Adam Arico, Brian Fiala, Robert F. Goldberg & Shaun Nichols - 2011 - Mind and Language 26 (3):327-352.
    This paper proposes the ‘AGENCY model’ of conscious state attribution, according to which an entity's displaying certain relatively simple features (e.g. eyes, distinctive motions, interactive behavior) automatically triggers a disposition to attribute conscious states to that entity. To test the model's predictions, participants completed a speeded object/attribution task, in which they responded positively or negatively to attributions of mental properties (including conscious and non-conscious states) to different sorts of entities (insects, plants, artifacts, etc.). As predicted, participants responded positively to conscious (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Matters of definition in the demystification of mental imagery.John S. Antrobus - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):549-550.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Integrating experiential and distributional data to learn semantic representations.Mark Andrews, Gabriella Vigliocco & David Vinson - 2009 - Psychological Review 116 (3):463-498.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • Head-centered coordinates and the stable feature frame.Richard A. Andersen - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):289-290.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Anthropology in Cognitive Science.Andrea Bender, Edwin Hutchins & Douglas Medin - 2010 - Topics in Cognitive Science 2 (3):374-385.
    This paper reviews the uneven history of the relationship between Anthropology and Cognitive Science over the past 30 years, from its promising beginnings, followed by a period of disaffection, on up to the current context, which may lay the groundwork for reconsidering what Anthropology and (the rest of) Cognitive Science have to offer each other. We think that this history has important lessons to teach and has implications for contemporary efforts to restore Anthropology to its proper place within Cognitive Science. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • A Two‐Stage Model of Category Construction.Woo-Kyoung Ahn & Douglas L. Medin - 1992 - Cognitive Science 16 (1):81-121.
    The current consensus is that most natural categories are not organized around strict definitions (a list of singly necessary and jointly sufficient features) but rather according to a family resemblance (FR) principle: Objects belong to the same category because they are similar to each other and dissimilar to objects in contrast categories. A number of computational models of category construction have been developed to provide an account of how and why people create FR categories (Anderson, 1990; Fisher, 1987). Surprisingly, however, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Imagining the purpose of imagery.Robert P. Abelson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):548-549.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Differences Between Belief and Knowledge Systems.Robert P. Abelson - 1979 - Cognitive Science 3 (4):355-366.
    Seven features which in practice seem to differentiate belief systems from knowledge systems are discussed. These are: nonconsensuality, “existence beliefs,” alternative worlds, evaluative components, episodic material, unboundedness, and variable credences. Each of these features gives rise to challenging representation problems. Progress on any of these problems within artificial intelligence would be helpful in the study of knowledge systems as well as belief systems, inasmuch as the distinction between the two types of systems is not absolute.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Inference and the structure of concepts.Matías Osta Vélez - 2020 - Dissertation, Ludwig Maximilians Universität, München
    This thesis studies the role of conceptual content in inference and reasoning. The first two chapters offer a theoretical and historical overview of the relation between inference and meaning in philosophy and psychology. In particular, a critical analysis of the formality thesis, i.e., the idea that rational inference is a rule-based and topic-neutral mechanism, is advanced. The origins of this idea in logic and its influence in philosophy and cognitive psychology are discussed. Chapter 3 consists of an analysis of the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Word meaning.Luca Gasparri & Diego Marconi - 2015 - Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Extended Artificial Memory. Toward an integral cognitive theory of memory and technology.Lars Ludwig - unknown
    This thesis is a contribution toward an integral cognitive theory of memory and technology. It, furthermore, develops a theory and prototype for technologically extending mind (memory and thinking).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Inteligencia y sistema cognitivo.Emilio García García - 1996 - Anales Del Seminario de Historia de la Filosofía 13 (S1):445-462.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Concepts and Cognitive Science.Stephen Laurence & Eric Margolis - 1999 - In Eric Margolis & Stephen Laurence (eds.), Concepts: Core Readings. MIT Press. pp. 3-81.
    Given the fundamental role that concepts play in theories of cognition, philosophers and cognitive scientists have a common interest in concepts. Nonetheless, there is a great deal of controversy regarding what kinds of things concepts are, how they are structured, and how they are acquired. This chapter offers a detailed high-level overview and critical evaluation of the main theories of concepts and their motivations. Taking into account the various challenges that each theory faces, the chapter also presents a novel approach (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   247 citations  
  • Regaining composure: A defense of prototype compositionality.Jesse Prinz - manuscript
    Beginning in the late 1960s, psychologists began to challenge the view the definitional theory of concepts. According to that theory a concept is a mental representation comprising representations of properties (or “features”) that are individually necessary and jointly sufficient for membership in a category. In place of the definitional view, psychologists initially put forward the prototype theory of concept, according to which concepts comprise representations of features that are typical, salient, and diagnostic for category membership, but not necessarily necessary. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Nonintentional similarity processing.Arthur B. Markman & Dedre Gentner - 2005 - In Ran R. Hassin, James S. Uleman & John A. Bargh (eds.), The New Unconscious. Oxford Series in Social Cognition and Social Neuroscience. Oxford University Press. pp. 107--137.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Life Cycle of a Concept in the Ad Hoc Cognition Framework.José Vicente Hernández-Conde - 2017 - Theoria : An International Journal for Theory, History and Fundations of Science 32 (3).
    Recently, Casasanto and Lupyan have asserted that there are no context-independent concepts: all concepts are constructed ad hoc when they are instantiated. My aim is to show that the ad hoc cognition framework can be characterized by a similarity-based theory of concepts, and that two different notions of concept should be distinguished —which may be identified with two distinct stages of their life cycle. This approach brings together virtues from opposing views: invariantist: stored concepts are stable enough to collect new (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Analyzing the factors underlying the structure and computation of the meaning of< em> chipmunk,< em> cherry,< em> chisel,< em> cheese, and< em> cello(and many other such concrete nouns).George S. Cree & Ken McRae - 2003 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 132 (2):163.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Moral philosophy and mental representation.Stephen Stich - 1993 - In R. Michod, L. Nadel & M. Hechter (eds.), The Origin of Values. Aldine de Gruyer. pp. 215--228.
    Here is an overview of what is to come. In Sections I and II, I will sketch two of the projects frequently pursued by moral philosophers, and the methods typically invoked in those projects. I will argue that these projects presuppose (or at least suggest) a particular sort of account of the mental representation of human value systems, since the methods make sense only if we assume a certain kind of story about how the human mind stores information about values. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations