Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The cerebellum and memory.Richard F. Thompson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):801-802.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex.J. F. Stein - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):691-700.
    The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) is the most likely site where egocentric spatial relationships are represented in the brain. PPC cells receive visual, auditory, somaesthetic, and vestibular sensory inputs; oculomotor, head, limb, and body motor signals; and strong motivational projections from the limbic system. Their discharge increases not only when an animal moves towards a sensory target, but also when it directs its attention to it. PPC lesions have the opposite effect: sensory inattention and neglect. The PPC does not seem (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   87 citations  
  • Joinings, discontinuities and details: Darwin, sex and status revisited.Jerome H. Barkow - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):320-334.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Potential difficulties in the evaluation of motor strategies using EMG patterns.Warren G. Darling - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):352-353.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Strategies for goal-directed fast movements are byproducts of satisfying performance criteria.Jack M. Winters & Amir H. Seif-Naraghi - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):357-359.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The reemergence of evolutionary psychology?Charles Crawford & Tracy Lindberg - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):305-305.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Bursts of discharge recorded from the red nucleus may provide real measures of Gottlieb's excitation pulses.James C. Houk - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):224-225.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Degrees of freedom, dynamical laws, and boundary conditions for discrete voluntary movement.J. A. S. Kelso - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):225-225.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On to real-life movements.Paul J. Cordo, Fay B. Horak & Susan P. Moore - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):214-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Strategies for the control of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom.Gerald L. Gottlieb, Daniel M. Corcos & Gyan C. Agarwal - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):189-210.
    A theory is presented to explain how accurate, single-joint movements are controlled. The theory applies to movements across different distances, with different inertial loads, toward targets of different widths over a wide range of experimentally manipulated velocities. The theory is based on three propositions. (1) Movements are planned according to “strategies” of which there are at least two: a speed-insensitive (SI) and a speed-sensitive (SS) one. (2) These strategies can be equated with sets of rules for performing diverse movement tasks. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   183 citations  
  • Can voluntary movement be understood on the basis of reflex organization?David J. Ostry & Frances E. Wilkinson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):618-619.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Origins of origins of motor control.Esther Thelen - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):780-783.
    Examination of infant spontaneous and goal-directed arm movements supports Feldman and Levin's hypothesis of a functional hierarchy. Early infant movements are dominated by biomechanical and dynamic factors without external frames of reference. Development involves not only learning to generate these frames of reference, but also protecting the higher-level goal of the movement from internal and external perturbations.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How far should we extend the equilibrium point (lambda) hypothesis?Jack M. Winters - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):785-786.
    A key feature of the lambda model is the hypothesis of a local spring-like muscle-reflex system defined by a central control variable that has units of position. This is intriguing, especially for a study of postural stability in large-scale systems, but it has limited direct application to skilled everyday movements. If movement is considered as a goal-directed, neuro-optimization problem, however, theavailabilityof lambda-like peripheral models (vs. conventional musculoskeletal models) deserves exploration.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Spatial frames for motor control would be commensurate with spatial frames for vision and proprioception, but what about control of energy flows?Christopher C. Pagano & Geoffrey P. Bingham - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):773-773.
    The model identifies a spatial coordinate frame within which the sensorimotor apparatus produces movement. Its spatial nature simplifies its coupling with spatial reference frames used concurrently by vision and proprioception. While the positional reference frame addresses the performance of spatial tasks, it seems to have little to say about movements involving energy expenditure as the principle component of the task.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The λ model: Can it walk?Aftab E. Patla - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):775-776.
    Generation of swing phase limb trajectory over obstacles during locomotion should be a reasonable test for the λ model proposed by Feldman and Levin. The observed features such as lack of simple amplitude scaling of endpoint (toe) trajectories for different obstacle heights, complex shaped toe velocity profiles, and exploitation of passive intersegmental dynamics to control limb elevation cannot be adequately explained by the λ model.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Two joints are more than twice one joint.Jeroen B. J. Smeets - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):779-780.
    An alternative multi-joint extension to the lambda model is proposed. According to this extension, the activity of a muscle depends not only on the difference between lambda and length of that muscle, but also on the difference between lambda and length of other muscles. This 2-D extension can describe more neurophysiological experiments than the extension proposed in the target article.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The unobservability of central commands: Why testing hypotheses is so difficult.Antony Hodgson - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):763-764.
    The experiments Feldman and Levin suggest do not definitively test their proposed solution to the problem of selecting muscle activations. Their test of the movement directions that elicit EMG activity can be interpreted without regard to the form of the central commands, and their fast elbow flexion test is based on a forward computation that obscures the insensitivity of the predicted trajectory to the details of the putative commands.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Inverse kinematic problem: Solutions by pseudoinversion, inversion and no-inversion.Simon R. Goodman - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):756-758.
    Kinematic properties of reaching movements reflect constraints imposed on the joint angles. Contemporary models present solutions to the redundancy problem by a pseudoinverse procedure (Whitney 1969) or without any inversion (Berkenblit et al. 1986). Feldman & Levin suggest a procedure based on a regular inversion. These procedures are considered as an outcome of a more general approach.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Twisted pairs: Does the motor system really care about joint configurations?Patrick Haggard, Chris Miall & John Stein - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):758-761.
    Extrapersonal frames of reference for aimed movements are representationally convenient. They may, however, carry associated costs when the movement is executed in terms of the complex coordination of multiple joints they require. Studies that have measured both fingertip and joint paths suggest the motor systems may seek a compromise between simplicity of extrapersonal spatial representation and computational simplicity of multi-joint execution.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Shifting frames of reference but the same old point of view.Gerald L. Gottlieb - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):758-758.
    Models of central control variables (CVs) that are expressed in positional reference frames and rely on proprioception as the dominant specifier of muscle activation patterns have not yet been shown to be adequate for the description of fast, voluntary movement, even of single joints. An alternative model with illustrative data is proposed.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Task analysis of a style of behavior.Peter H. Greene - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):155-155.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Successive approximation in targeted movement: An alternative hypothesis.Paul J. Cordo & Leslie Bevan - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):729-730.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Maladaptation and hierarchically organized explanatory levels.Ronald C. Simons - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):314-315.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does constraining movements constrain the developement of movement theories?Daniel M. Corcos, Gerland L. Gottlieb & Gyan C. Agarwal - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):237-250.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • If a particular strategy is used, what aspects of the movement are controlled?C. C. A. M. Gielen & J. J. Denier van der Gon - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):218-219.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Speed-insensitive and speed-sensitive strategies in multijoint movements.Tamar Flash - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):215-216.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Motor equivalence and goal descriptors.Kevin G. Munhall - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):615-616.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are posture and movement different expressions of the same mechanisms?R. M. Enoka - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):602-603.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do control variables exist?Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos & William H. Warren - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):762-762.
    We argue that the concept of a control variable (CV) as described by Feldman and Levin needs to be revised because it does not account for the influence of sensory feedback from the periphery. We provide evidence from the realm of rhythmic movements that sensory feedback can permanently alter the frequency and phase of a centrally generated rhythm.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What can we expect from models of motor control?Gerald E. Loeb - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):767-768.
    The lambda model of servocontrol seems superior to the alpha model in terms of dealing with the mechanical complexities of nonlinear and multiarticular muscles. Both, however, can be trivialized by noting that the “control variable” may simply be the sum of descending influences at propriospinal interneurons in the case of the lambda model or in the muscles themselves in the case of the alpha model. The notion that the brain explicitly computes output in terms of any such control variables may (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A few reasons why psychologlsts can adhere to Feldman and Levin's model.Mireille Bonnard & Jean Pailhous - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):746-747.
    We emphasize the relevance to cognitive psychology of Feldman and Levin's theoretical position. Traditional views of motor control have failed to clearly separate “production control” at the level of motor command, based on task-independent CV (control variables), from intentional “product control” based on task-dependent parameters. Because F&L's approach concentrates on the first process (trajectory formation), it can distinguish the product control stage.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Equilibrium-point hypothesis, minimum effort control strategy and the triphasic muscle activation pattern.Ning Lan & Patrick E. Crago - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):769-771.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Human nature and the Holy Grail.Randolph M. Nesse - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):312-313.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Too many errors.Martin Daly - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):306-307.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • When the mind goes awry: Schizophrenia and the emergence of culture.Jay R. Feierman - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):307-308.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Folk psychology redux.Linnda R. Caporael - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):302-303.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Time optimality, proprioception, and the triphasic EMG pattern.Constance Ramos, Lawrence Stark & Blake Hannaford - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):231-232.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Movement strategies as points on equal-outcome curves.Herbert Heuer - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):220-221.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Strategies and motor programs.Bruce D. Burns & Jeffery J. Summers - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):214-214.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Simple changes in reflex threshold cannot explain all aspects of rapid voluntary movements.C. Gielen & J. C. Houk - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):605-607.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Levers to generate movement.U. Windhorst - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):784-785.
    The following questions are discussed: (1) Who determines the nature of “control variables”? (2) Is the “positional monopoly” healthy? (3) Does a descending command alter reflex threshold alone without eoncomitantly altering stiffness? (4) How does the CNS deal with history-dependent effects? (5) Should we abandon the idea that the CNS controls classical Newtonian variables such as muscle length?
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Grip force adjustments during rapid hand movements suggest that detailed movement kinematics are predicted.J. Randall Flanagan, James R. Tresilian & Alan M. Wing - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):753-754.
    The λ model suggests that detailed kinematics arise from changes in control variables and need not be explicitly planned. However, we have shown that when moving a grasped object, grip force is precisely modulated in phase with acceleration-dependent inertial load. This suggests that the motor system can predict detailed kinematics. This prediction may be based on a forward model of the dynamics of the loaded limb.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is the multi-joint pointing movement model applicable to equilibrium control during upper trunk movements?Alexey Alexandrov, Alexander Frolov & Jean Massion - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-746.
    Two aspects of the target article, (1) the extension of the equilibrium point theory to multi-joint movements, and (2) the consequence that the EMG pattern is not directly controlled by the central nervous system (CNS), are discussed in light of the experiments on upper trunk bending in humans. The principle component kinematic analysis and the analysis of the EMG data, obtained under microgravity and additional loading conditions, support the application of Feldman and Levin's for multi-joint pointing movement to equilibrium control (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Should dynamic and passive properties be considered in analyses of human postural control?R. E. Kearney & I. W. Hunter - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):158-159.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Postural control: A further look at neural control stategies set by boundaries in space.Felix E. Zajac - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):167-167.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Biological variability and control of movements via δλ.Charles E. Wright & Rebecca A. States - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):786-786.
    Three issues related to Feldman and Levin's treatment of biological variability are discussed. We question the usefulness of the indirect component of δλ. We suggest that trade-offs between speed and accuracy in aimed movements support identification of δλ, rather than λ, as a control variable. We take issue with the authors' proposal for resolving redundancy in multi-joint movements, given recent data.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What is adapted in strategy-governed movements?U. Windhorst - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):236-237.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the hierarchy of “reflexes”.Uwe Windhorst - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):625-626.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Freud and sociobiology.N. E. Wetherick - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):319-320.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Why are “strategies’ senstitive? Smoothing the way for raison d'àtre”.John P. Wann, Ian Nimmo-Smith & Alan M. Wing - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):235-236.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark