- Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements?S. C. Gandevia & David Burke - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):614-632.details
|
|
Implications of neural networks for how we think about brain function.David A. Robinson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):644-655.details
|
|
When the mind goes awry: Schizophrenia and the emergence of culture.Jay R. Feierman - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):307-308.details
|
|
Person schemas: Evolutionary, individual developmental and social sources.Mardi J. Horowitz - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):309-310.details
|
|
Strategies and motor programs.Bruce D. Burns & Jeffery J. Summers - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):214-214.details
|
|
Skeletal and oculomotor control systems compared.Bruce Bridgeman - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):212-212.details
|
|
Frogs solve Bernstein's problem.Lloyd D. Partridge - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):619-620.details
|
|
Motor equivalence and goal descriptors.Kevin G. Munhall - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):615-616.details
|
|
Is λ an appropriate control variable for locomotion?Thomas M. Hamm & Zong-Sheng Han - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):761-762.details
|
|
The unobservability of central commands: Why testing hypotheses is so difficult.Antony Hodgson - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):763-764.details
|
|
Grip force adjustments during rapid hand movements suggest that detailed movement kinematics are predicted.J. Randall Flanagan, James R. Tresilian & Alan M. Wing - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):753-754.details
|
|
Reciprocal and coactivation commands are not sufficient to describe muscle activation patterns.C. C. A. M. Gielen & B. van Bolhuis - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):754-755.details
|
|
The organization of human postural movements: a formal basis and experimental synthesis.Lewis M. Nashner & Gin McCollum - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):135-150.details
|
|
Maladaptation and hierarchically organized explanatory levels.Ronald C. Simons - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):314-315.details
|
|
The reemergence of evolutionary psychology?Charles Crawford & Tracy Lindberg - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):305-305.details
|
|
The awakened brain: From Wright's psychozoology to Barkow's selfless persons.David Paul Lumsden - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):311-312.details
|
|
Does constraining movements constrain the developement of movement theories?Daniel M. Corcos, Gerland L. Gottlieb & Gyan C. Agarwal - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):237-250.details
|
|
What is adapted in strategy-governed movements?U. Windhorst - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):236-237.details
|
|
Strategies are a means to an end.C. Ghez & J. Gordon - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):216-218.details
|
|
On to real-life movements.Paul J. Cordo, Fay B. Horak & Susan P. Moore - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):214-215.details
|
|
Complexity in control of movements.Gyan C. Agarwal & Gerald L. Gottlieb - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):599-600.details
|
|
How far should we extend the equilibrium point (lambda) hypothesis?Jack M. Winters - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):785-786.details
|
|
Interneurons as backseat drivers and the elusive control variable.T. Richard Nichols - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):772-773.details
|
|
Can the λ model benefit from understanding human adaptation in weightlessness(and vice versa)?P. Vernon McDonald - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):768-768.details
|
|
Do control variables exist?Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos & William H. Warren - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):762-762.details
|
|
Shifting frames of reference but the same old point of view.Gerald L. Gottlieb - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):758-758.details
|
|
Identifying units of motor behavior.Richard A. Schmidt - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):163-164.details
|
|
Simplifying assumptions: Can development help?Esther Thelen - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):165-166.details
|
|
Anatomical asymmetry and boundary crossings in postural control.George E. Stelmach & Charles Worringham - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):164-165.details
|
|
The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex.J. F. Stein - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):691-700.details
|
|
Movement strategies and the necessity for task differentiation.Daniel M. Corcos, Simon R. Gutman, Gyan C. Agarwal & Gerald L. Gottlieb - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):359-364.details
|
|
Strategies for goal-directed fast movements are byproducts of satisfying performance criteria.Jack M. Winters & Amir H. Seif-Naraghi - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):357-359.details
|
|
Two ways to reduce motor programming load.Dennis H. Holding - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):224-224.details
|
|
Braking may be more critical than acceleration.William A. MacKay - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):227-228.details
|
|
What are the building blocks of the frog's wiping reflex?Ilan Golani - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):607-608.details
|
|
Equifinality and phase-resetting: The role of control parameter manipulations.R. E. A. van Emmerik & R. C. Wagenaar - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):783-784.details
|
|
The lambda model is only one piece in the motor control puzzle.Jeffrey Dean - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):749-749.details
|
|
Is the multi-joint pointing movement model applicable to equilibrium control during upper trunk movements?Alexey Alexandrov, Alexander Frolov & Jean Massion - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-746.details
|
|
The origin and use of positional frames of reference in motor control.Anatol G. Feldman & Mindy F. Levin - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):723-744.details
|
|
The role of learning in sensory-motor control.Stephen Grossberg - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):155-157.details
|
|
Less cybernetics, more geometry….René Thom - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):166-167.details
|
|
Bernsteinian physiology and computational modeling: East meets West at the “boundary”.Gary Goldberg & Hon C. Kwan - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):153-154.details
|
|
Should dynamic and passive properties be considered in analyses of human postural control?R. E. Kearney & I. W. Hunter - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):158-159.details
|
|
Joinings, discontinuities and details: Darwin, sex and status revisited.Jerome H. Barkow - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):320-334.details
|
|
Elementary conditions for elemental movement strategies.Charles B. Walter - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):234-235.details
|
|
If a particular strategy is used, what aspects of the movement are controlled?C. C. A. M. Gielen & J. J. Denier van der Gon - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):218-219.details
|
|
In search of the theoretical basis of motor control.M. B. Berkinblit, A. G. Feldman & O. I. Fukson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):626-638.details
|
|
Levers to generate movement.U. Windhorst - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):784-785.details
|
|
Kinematic invariances and body schema.Pietro Morasso & Vittorio Sanguineti - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):769-770.details
|
|
The control of sets of muscles: A general principle?Fred Delcomyn - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):153-153.details
|
|