Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Above and beyond the concrete: The diverse representational substrates of the predictive brain.Michael Gilead, Yaacov Trope & Nira Liberman - 2020 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 43:e121.
    In recent years, scientists have increasingly taken to investigate the predictive nature of cognition. We argue that prediction relies on abstraction, and thus theories of predictive cognition need an explicit theory of abstract representation. We propose such a theory of the abstract representational capacities that allow humans to transcend the “here-and-now.” Consistent with the predictive cognition literature, we suggest that the representational substrates of the mind are built as ahierarchy, ranging from the concrete to the abstract; however, we argue that (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Far-Persons.Gary Comstock - 2017 - In Woodhall Andrew & Garmendia da Trindade Gabriel (eds.), Ethics and/or Politics: Approaching the Issues Concerning Nonhuman Animals. Palgrave. pp. 39-71.
    I argue for the moral relevance of a category of individuals I characterize as far-persons. Following Gary Varner, I distinguish near-persons, animals with a " robust autonoetic consciousness " but lacking an adult human's " biographical sense of self, " from the merely sentient, those animals living "entirely in the present." I note the possibility of a third class. Far-persons lack a biographical sense of self, possess a weak autonoetic consciousness, and are able to travel mentally through time a distance (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Decisions and the evolution of memory: Multiple systems, multiple functions.Stanley B. Klein, Leda Cosmides, John Tooby & Sarah Chance - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (2):306-329.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • The Episodicity of Memory: Current Trends and Issues in Philosophy and Psychology.D. Perrin & S. Rousset - 2014 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 5 (3):291-312.
    Although episodic memory is a widely studied form of memory both in philosophy and psychology, it still raises many burning questions regarding its definition and even its acceptance. Over the last two decades, cross-disciplinary discussions between these two fields have increased as they tackle shared concerns, such as the phenomenology of recollection, and therefore allow for fruitful interaction. This editorial introduction aims to provide a comprehensive and up-to-date presentation of the main existing conceptions and issues on the topic. After delineating (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Episodic and semantic memory: Where should we go from here?Endel Tulving - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):573-577.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Theories of visual masking.Bruce Bridgeman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):25-26.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Knowing and knowing you know: Better methods or better models?Ira Fischler - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):32-33.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ecphoria excelsa.Michael J. Watkins - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):787.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The role of vocalization, memory retrieval, and external symbols in cognitive evolution.Merlin Donald - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):159-164.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How to decide whether a neural representation is a cognitive concept?Maartje E. J. Raijmakers & Peter C. M. Molenaar - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):641-642.
    A distinction should be made between the formation of stimulus-driven associations and cognitive concepts. To test the learning mode of a neural network, we propose a simple and classic input-output test: the discrimination shift task. Feed-forward PDP models appear to form stimulus-driven associations. A Hopfield network should be extended to apply the test.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Additional tests of Amit's attractor neural networks.Ralph E. Hoffman - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):634-635.
    Further tests of Amit's model are indicated. One strategy is to use the apparent coding sparseness of the model to make predictions about coding sparseness in Miyashita's network. A second approach is to use memory overload to induce false positive responses in modules and biological systems. In closing, the importance of temporal coding and timing requirements in developing biologically plausible attractor networks is mentioned.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Memory processing by the brain: Subregionalization, species-dependency, and network character.Hans J. Markowitsch - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):506-507.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Précis of Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition.Merlin Donald - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):737-748.
    This bold and brilliant book asks the ultimate question of the life sciences: How did the human mind acquire its incomparable power? In seeking the answer, Merlin Donald traces the evolution of human culture and cognition from primitive apes to the era of artificial intelligence, and presents an original theory of how the human mind evolved from its presymbolic form. In the emergence of modern human culture, Donald proposes, there were three radical transitions. During the first, our bipedal but still (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   335 citations  
  • What has episodic memory got to do with space and time?Ian Phillips - forthcoming - In Lynn Nadel & Sara Aronowitz (eds.), Space, Time, and Memory. Oxford University Press.
    It is widely held that episodic memory is constitutively connected with space and time. In particular, many contend that episodic memory constitutively has spatial and/or temporal content: for instance, necessarily representing a spatial scene, or when a given event occurred, or at the very minimum that it occurred in the past. Here, I critically assess such claims. I begin with some preparatory remarks on the nature of episodic memory. I then ask: How, if at all, is episodic memory constitutively spatial? (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The impure phenomenology of episodic memory.Alexandria Boyle - 2019 - Mind and Language 35 (5):641-660.
    Episodic memory has a distinctive phenomenology: it involves “mentally reliving” a past event. It has been suggested that characterising episodic memory in terms of this phenomenology makes it impossible to test for in animals, because “purely phenomenological features” cannot be detected in animal behaviour. Against this, I argue that episodic memory's phenomenological features are impure, having both subjective and objective aspects, and so can be behaviourally detected. Insisting on a phenomenological characterisation of episodic memory consequently does nothing to damage the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • The psychophysics of subliminal perception.Neil A. Macmillan - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):38-39.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • What do you mean by conscious?John Morton - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):43-43.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Hunting memes.H. C. Plotkin - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):768-769.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cultural transitions occur when mind parasites learn new tricks.Liane M. Gabora - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):760-761.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Episodic is what apes are not.Keith Stenning - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):158-159.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mathematics of Hebbian attractors.Morris W. Hirsch - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):633-634.
    The concept of an attractor in a mathematical dynamical system is reviewed. Emphasis is placed on the distinction between a cell assembly, the corresponding attractor, and the attractor dynamics. The biological significance of these entities is discussed, especially the question of whether the representation of the stimulus requires the full attractor dynamics, or merely the cell assembly as a set of reverberating neurons. Comparison is made to Freeman's study of dynamic patterns in olfaction.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What's in a cell assembly?G. J. Dalenoort & P. H. de Vries - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):629-630.
    The cell assembly as a simple attractor cannot explain many cognitive phenomena. It must be a highly structured network that can sustain highly structured excitation patterns. Moreover, a cell assembly must be more widely distributed in space than on a square millimeter.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Personal semantics: at the crossroads of semantic and episodic memory.Louis Renoult, Patrick Sr Davidson, Daniela J. Palombo, Morris Moscovitch & Brian Levine - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (11):550-558.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Memory buffer and comparator can share the same circuitry.J. A. Gray - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):501-501.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Does our behavioral methodology conceal the deficit caused by hippocampal damage?David T. D. James - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):502-503.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Discontiguity and memory.David S. Olton - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):510-511.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans?Thomas Suddendorf & Michael C. Corballis - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (3):299-313.
    In a dynamic world, mechanisms allowing prediction of future situations can provide a selective advantage. We suggest that memory systems differ in the degree of flexibility they offer for anticipatory behavior and put forward a corresponding taxonomy of prospection. The adaptive advantage of any memory system can only lie in what it contributes for future survival. The most flexible is episodic memory, which we suggest is part of a more general faculty of mental time travel that allows us not only (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   308 citations  
  • The meaning of “time” in episodic memory and mental time travel.William J. Friedman - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (3):323-323.
    The role of time in episodic memory and mental time travel is considered in light of findings on humans' temporal memory and anticipation. Time is not integral or uniform in memory for the past or anticipation of the future. The commonalities of episodic memory and anticipation require further study.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal.Daniel Holender - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):1-23.
    When the stored representation of the meaning of a stimulus is accessed through the processing of a sensory input it is maintained in an activated state for a certain amount of time that allows for further processing. This semantic activation is generally accompanied by conscious identification, which can be demonstrated by the ability of a person to perform discriminations on the basis of the meaning of the stimulus. The idea that a sensory input can give rise to semantic activation without (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   449 citations  
  • Semantic and affective manifestations of ambi.Oksana Itkes, Zohar Eviatar & Assaf Kron - 2019 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (7):1356-1369.
    ABSTRACTPeople sometimes report both pleasant and unpleasant feelings when presented with affective stimuli. However, what is reported as “mixed emotions” might reflect semantic knowledge about the...
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Emotion in languaging: languaging as affective, adaptive, and flexible behavior in social interaction.Thomas W. Jensen - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5:96268.
    This article argues for a view on languaging as inherently affective. Informed by recent ecological tendencies within cognitive science and distributed language studies a distinction between first order languaging (language as whole-body sense making) and second order language (language as system like constraints) is put forward. Contrary to common assumptions within linguistics and communication studies separating language-as-a-system from language use (resulting in separations between language vs. body-language and verbal vs. non-verbal communication etc.) the first/second order distinction sees language as emanating (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Explaining “virtuoso” hypnotic performance: Social psychology or experiential skill?Kenneth R. Graham - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):473-474.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Attentional orienting precedes conscious identification.Albrecht Werner Inhoff - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):35-35.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Consciousness, causality and complementarity.Max Velmans - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):404-416.
    This reply to five continuing commentaries on my 1991 target article on “Is human information processing conscious” focuses on six related issues: 1) whether focal attentive processing replaces consciousness as a causal agent in third-person viewable human information processing, 2)whether consciousness can be dissociated from human information processing, 3) continuing disputes about definitions of "consciousness" and about what constitutes a “conscious process” , 4) how observer-relativity in psychology relates (and does not relate) to relativity in physics, 5) whether the first-person (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   48 citations  
  • Archaeological evidence for mimetic mind and culture.Thomas Wynn - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):774-774.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Human evolution: Emergence of the group-self.Vilmos Csányi - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):755-756.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Working memory and its extensions.K. J. Gilhooly - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):761-762.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From cooperative computation to man/machine symbiosis.Michael A. Arbib - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):748-749.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Symbolic invention: The missing (computational) link?Andy Clark - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):753-754.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Epistemology of Geometry I: the Problem of Exactness.Anne Newstead & Franklin James - 2010 - Proceedings of the Australasian Society for Cognitive Science 2009.
    We show how an epistemology informed by cognitive science promises to shed light on an ancient problem in the philosophy of mathematics: the problem of exactness. The problem of exactness arises because geometrical knowledge is thought to concern perfect geometrical forms, whereas the embodiment of such forms in the natural world may be imperfect. There thus arises an apparent mismatch between mathematical concepts and physical reality. We propose that the problem can be solved by emphasizing the ways in which the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Comparing human evaluations of eyewitness statements to a machine learning classifier under pristine and suboptimal lineup administration procedures.Jesse H. Grabman, Ian G. Dobbins & Chad S. Dodson - 2024 - Cognition 251 (C):105876.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Affective and Semantic Representations of Valence: A Conceptual Framework.Oksana Itkes & Assaf Kron - 2019 - Emotion Review 11 (4):283-293.
    The current article discusses the distinction between affective valence—the degree to which an affective response represents pleasure or displeasure—and semantic valence, the degree to which an object or event is considered positive or negative. To date, measures that reflect positivity and negativity are usually placed under the same conceptual umbrella (e.g., valence, affective, emotional), with minimal distinction between the modes of valence they reflect. Recent work suggests that what might seem to reflect a monolithic structure of valence has at least (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Hypnotic behavior: A social-psychological interpretation of amnesia, analgesia, and “trance logic”.Nicholas P. Spanos - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):449-467.
    This paper examines research on three hypnotic phenomena: suggested amnesia, suggested analgesia, and “trance logic.” For each case a social-psychological interpretation of hypnotic behavior as a voluntary response strategy is compared with the traditional special-process view that “good” hypnotic subjects have lost conscious control over suggestion-induced behavior. I conclude that it is inaccurate to describe hypnotically amnesic subjects as unable to recall the material they have been instructed to forget. Although amnesics present themselves as unable to remember, they in fact (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   129 citations  
  • The evolved mind.Harry J. Jerison - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):763-764.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Apes have mimetic culture.Robert W. Mitchell & H. Lyn Miles - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):768-768.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Mimetic culture and modern sports: A synthesis.Bruce Bridgeman & Margarita Azmitia - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):751-752.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How representation works is more important than what representations are.Shimon Edelman - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):630-631.
    A theory of representation is incomplete if it states “representations areX” whereXcan be symbols, cell assemblies, functional states, or the flock of birds fromTheaetetus, without explaining the nature of the link between the universe ofXs and the world. Amit's thesis, equating representations with reverberations in Hebbian cell assemblies, will only be considered a solution to the problem of representation when it is complemented by a theory of how a reverberation in the brain can be a representation of anything.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The emotional content of life stories: Positivity bias and relation to personality.Dorthe Kirkegaard Thomsen, Martin Hammershøj Olesen, Anette Schnieber & Jan Tønnesvang - 2014 - Cognition and Emotion 28 (2):260-277.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A physiological basis for hippocampal involvement in coding temporally discontiguous events.Sam A. Deadwyler - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):500-501.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations