Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Reverberations of Hebbian thinking.Josef P. Rauschecker - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):642-643.
    Cortical reverberations may induce synaptic changes that underlie developmental plasticity as well as long-term memory. They may be especially important for the consolidation of synaptic changes. Reverberations in cortical networks should have particular significance during development, when large numbers of new representations are formed. This includes the formation of representations across different sensory modalities.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The functional meaning of reverberations for sensoric and contextual encoding.Wolfgang Klimesch - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):636-636.
    Amit argues that the local neuronal spike rate that persists (reverberating) in the absence of the eliciting stimulus represents the code of the eliciting stimulus. Based on the general argument that the inferred functional meaning of reverberation depends in part on the type of representational assumptions, reverberations may only be important for the encoding of contextual information.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • An evolutionary perspective on Hebb's reverberatory representations.David C. Krakauer & Alasdair I. Houston - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):636-637.
    Hebbian mechanisms are justified according to their functional utility in an evolutionary sense. The selective advantage of correlating content-contingent stimuli reflects the putative common cause of temporally or spatially contiguous inputs. The selective consequences of such correlations are discussed by using examples from the evolution of signal form in sexual selection and model-mimic coevolution. We suggest that evolutionary justification might be considered in addition to neurophysiology plansibility when constructing representational models.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Distributed cell assemblies and detailed cell models.Anders Lansner & Erik Fransén - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):637-638.
    Hebbian cell-assembly theory and attractor networks are good starting points for modeling cortical processing. Detailed cell models can be useful in understanding the dynamics of attractor networks. Cell assemblies are likely to be distributed, with the cortical column as the local processing unit. Synaptic memory may be dominant in all but the first couple of seconds.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Attractors – don't get sucked in.Peter M. Milner - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):638-639.
    Every immediate memory is unique; it is therefore unlikely to consist of an attractor or even a combination of attractors. In the present state of knowledge about the chemistry of synaptic transmission, there is no reason to look beyond neurons that directly receive sensory afferents for the afterdischarges that correspond to active memories.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Hebbian paradigm reintegrated: Local reverberations as internal representations.Walter J. Freeman - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):631-631.
    Recurrent excitation is experimentally well documented in cortical populations. It provides for intracortical excitatory biases that linearize negative feedback interactions and induce macroscopic state transitions during perception. The concept of the local neighborhood should be expanded to spatial patterns as the basis for perception, in which large areas of cortex are bound into cooperative behavior with near-silent columns as important as active columns revealed by unit recording.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Not the module does memory make – but the network.Joaquin M. Fuster - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):631-633.
    This commentary questions the target articles inferences from a limited set of empirical data to support this model and conceptual scheme. Especially questionable is the attribution of internal representation properties to an assembly of cells in a discrete cortical module firing at a discrete attractor frequency. Alternative inferences are drawn from cortical cooling and cell-firing data that point to the internal representation as a broad and specific cortical network defined by cortico-cortical connectivity. Active memory, it is proposed, consists in the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mathematics of Hebbian attractors.Morris W. Hirsch - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):633-634.
    The concept of an attractor in a mathematical dynamical system is reviewed. Emphasis is placed on the distinction between a cell assembly, the corresponding attractor, and the attractor dynamics. The biological significance of these entities is discussed, especially the question of whether the representation of the stimulus requires the full attractor dynamics, or merely the cell assembly as a set of reverberating neurons. Comparison is made to Freeman's study of dynamic patterns in olfaction.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Additional tests of Amit's attractor neural networks.Ralph E. Hoffman - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):634-635.
    Further tests of Amit's model are indicated. One strategy is to use the apparent coding sparseness of the model to make predictions about coding sparseness in Miyashita's network. A second approach is to use memory overload to induce false positive responses in modules and biological systems. In closing, the importance of temporal coding and timing requirements in developing biologically plausible attractor networks is mentioned.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are single-cell data sufficient for testing neural network models?Ehud Ahissar - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):626-627.
    Persistent activity can be the product of mechanisms other than attractor reverberations. The single-unit data presented by Amit cannot discriminate between the different mechanisms. In fact, single-unit data do not appear to be adequate for testing neural network models.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Where the adventure is.Elie Bienenstock & Stuart Geman - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):627-628.
    Interpreting the Miyashita et al. experiments in terms of a cellassembly representation does not adequately explain the performance of Miyashita's monkeys on novel stimuli. We will argue that the latter observations point to acompositionalrepresentation and suggest a dynamics involving rapid and reversible binding of distinct activity patterns.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Reverberation reconsidered: On the path to cognitive theory.Eric Chown - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):628-629.
    Amit's work addresses a critical issue in cognitive science: the structure of neural representations. The use of Hebbian cell assemblies is a positive step, and we now need to consider its role in a larger cognitive theory. When considering the dynamics of a system built out of attractors, a more limited version of reverberation becomes necessary.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What's in a cell assembly?G. J. Dalenoort & P. H. de Vries - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):629-630.
    The cell assembly as a simple attractor cannot explain many cognitive phenomena. It must be a highly structured network that can sustain highly structured excitation patterns. Moreover, a cell assembly must be more widely distributed in space than on a square millimeter.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Hebbian paradigm reintegrated: Local reverberations as internal representations.Daniel J. Amit - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):617-626.
    The neurophysiological evidence from the Miyashita group's experiments on monkeys as well as cognitive experience common to us all suggests that local neuronal spike rate distributions might persist in the absence of their eliciting stimulus. In Hebb's cell-assembly theory, learning dynamics stabilize such self-maintaining reverberations. Quasi-quantitive modeling of the experimental data on internal representations in association-cortex modules identifies the reverberations (delay spike activity) as the internal code (representation). This leads to cognitive and neurophysiological predictions, many following directly from the language (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • The emotional content of life stories: Positivity bias and relation to personality.Dorthe Kirkegaard Thomsen, Martin Hammershøj Olesen, Anette Schnieber & Jan Tønnesvang - 2014 - Cognition and Emotion 28 (2):260-277.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Mental representations of affect knowledge.Lisa Feldman Barrett & Thyra Fossum - 2001 - Cognition and Emotion 15 (3):333-363.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Memory on time.Howard Eichenbaum - 2013 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 (2):81-88.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Identification, masking, and priming: Clarifying the issues.Lindsay J. Evett, Glyn W. Humphreys & Philip T. Quinlan - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):31-32.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The development of theory: Logic of method or underlying processes?Charles P. Shimp - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):511-512.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Another hippocampal theory.Marc N. Branch - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):497-498.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Time and hippocampal lesion effects: Tempus edax rerum?J. N. P. Rawlins - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):514-528.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Does our behavioral methodology conceal the deficit caused by hippocampal damage?David T. D. James - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):502-503.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Three-store theories of memory.William S. Maki - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):505-506.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Temporal discontiguity: Alternative to, or component of, existing theories of hippocampal function?Donna J. Hughey - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):501-502.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Associations across time: The hippocampus as a temporary memory store.J. N. P. Rawlins - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):479-497.
    All recent memory theories of hippocampal function have incorporated the idea that the hippocampus is required to process items only of some qualitatively specifiahle kind, and is not required to process items of some complementary set. In contrast, it is now proposed that the hippocampus is needed to process stimuli of all kinds, but only when there is a need to associate those stimuli with other events that are temporally discontiguous. In order to form or use temporally discontiguous associations, it (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   142 citations  
  • Discontiguity and memory.David S. Olton - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):510-511.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Memory processing by the brain: Subregionalization, species-dependency, and network character.Hans J. Markowitsch - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):506-507.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is the hippocampus a store, intermediate or otherwise?Neil McNaughton - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):508-509.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The hippocampus and time.Gordon Winocur - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):512-513.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The hippocampus as episodic encoder: Does it play tag?Robert H. I. Dale - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):499-500.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A physiological basis for hippocampal involvement in coding temporally discontiguous events.Sam A. Deadwyler - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):500-501.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • Sharpening the focus on functions of the hippocampus.Daniel P. Kimble - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):504-505.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Effects of hippocampal lesions on some operant visual discrimination tasks.Michael L. Woodruff & Dennis L. Whittington - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):513-514.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Functional embodied imagination and episodic memory.Owen Holland & Hugo Gravato Marques - 2010 - International Journal of Machine Consciousness 2 (2):245-259.
    The phenomenon of episodic memory has been studied for over 30 years, but it is only recently that its constructive nature has been shown to be closely linked to the processes underpinning imagination. This paper builds on recent work by the authors in developing architectures for a form of imagination suitable for use in artifacts, and considers how these architectures might be extended to provide a form of episodic memory.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The mental time line: An analogue of the mental number line in the mapping of life events.Shahar Arzy, Esther Adi-Japha & Olaf Blanke - 2009 - Consciousness and Cognition 18 (3):781-785.
    A crucial aspect of the human mind is the ability to project the self along the time line to past and future. It has been argued that such self-projection is essential to re-experience past experiences and predict future events. In-depth analysis of a novel paradigm investigating mental time shows that the speed of this “self-projection” in time depends logarithmically on the temporal-distance between an imagined “location” on the time line that participants were asked to imagine and the location of another (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Did courtship drive the evolution of mind?Eric B. Baum - 1996 - Behavioural and Brain Sciences 19 (19):155-164.
    The driving force in the evolution of language and the human mind was the advantage gained in courtship by efficient communicators. Ontogeny recapitulating phylogeny may offer an alternative to Donald's picture of the relative origins of language and mimesis. Very recent evidence is pertinent to dating the origin of language.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark