Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. An evolutionary theory of schizophrenia: Cortical connectivity, metarepresentation, and the social brain.Jonathan Kenneth Burns - 2004 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (6):831-855.
    Schizophrenia is a worldwide, prevalent disorder with a multifactorial but highly genetic aetiology. A constant prevalence rate in the face of reduced fecundity has caused some to argue that an evolutionary advantage exists in unaffected relatives. Here, I critique this adaptationist approach, and review – and find wanting – Crow's “speciation” hypothesis. In keeping with available biological and psychological evidence, I propose an alternative theory of the origins of this disorder. Schizophrenia is a disorder of the social brain, and it (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • (2 other versions)Relevance theory.Deirdre Wilson & Dan Sperber - 2002 - In Deirdre Wilson & Dan Sperber (eds.), Relevance theory. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 607-632.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   160 citations  
  • Mind outside Brain: a radically non-dualist foundation for distributed cognition.Francis Heylighen & Shima Beigi - 2018 - In J. Adam Carter, Andy Clark, Jesper Kallestrup, S. Orestis Palermos & Duncan Pritchard (eds.), Socially Extended Epistemology. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. pp. 59-86.
    We approach the problem of the extended mind from a radically non-dualist perspective. The separation between mind and matter is an artefact of the outdated mechanistic worldview, which leaves no room for mental phenomena such as agency, intentionality, or feeling. We propose to replace it by an action ontology, which conceives mind and matter as aspects of the same network of processes. By adopting the intentional stance, we interpret the catalysts of elementary reactions as agents exhibiting desires, intentions, and sensations. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Counterfactual Theory of Free Will: A Genuinely Deterministic Form of Soft Determinism.Rick Repetti - 2010 - Saarbrücken, Germany: LAP Lambert Academic Publishing.
    I argue for a soft compatibilist theory of free will, i.e., such that free will is compatible with both determinism and indeterminism, directly opposite hard incompatibilism, which holds free will incompatible both with determinism and indeterminism. My intuitions in this book are primarily based on an analysis of meditation, but my arguments are highly syncretic, deriving from many fields, including behaviorism, psychology, conditioning and deconditioning theory, philosophy of language, philosophy of mind, simulation theory, etc. I offer a causal/functional analysis of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Social mirrors and shared experiential worlds.Charles Whitehead - 2001 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 8 (4):3-36.
    We humans have a formidable armamentarium of social display behaviours, including song-and-dance, the visual arts, and role-play. Of these, role-play is probably the crucial adaptation which makes us most different from other apes. Human childhood, a sheltered period of ‘extended irresponsibility’, allows us to develop our powers of make-believe and role-play, prerequisites for human cooperation, culture, and reflective consciousness. Social mirror theory, originating with Dilthey, Baldwin, Cooley and Mead, holds that there cannot be mirrors in the mind without mirrors in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Calls as labels: An intriguing theme, but one with limitations.Donald H. Owings - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):162-163.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Knowing thyself, knowing the other: They're not the same.Jonathan Schull & J. David Smith - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):166-167.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Monkeys and consciousness.D. M. Armstrong - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):147-148.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Working memory and its extensions.K. J. Gilhooly - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):761-762.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From mimesis to synthesis.Jerome A. Feldman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):759-759.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “Pop science” versus understanding the emergence of the modern mind.C. Loring Brace - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):750-751.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The place of cognition in human evolution.Alan Costall - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):755-755.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Culture, biology and human ontogeny.Michael Tomasello, Ann Gale Kruger & Hilary Horn Ratner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):540-552.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Moving forward on cultural learning.Angelina S. Lillard - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):528-529.
    Tomasello, Kruger & Ratner make the very interesting and valid point that the transmission of culture must depend on understanding others' minds. Culture is shared among a people and is passed on to progeny. The sharing of culture implies that the purpose of (and therefore the meaning behind) any given cultural element (behavioral tradition, word, or artifact) is understood. Because meaning or purpose emanates from minds, something about others' minds must be understood in order to truly learn some element of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cultural learning and educational process.David R. Olson & Janet Wilde Astington - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):531-532.
    Tomasello, Kruger & Ratner relate the evolution of social cognition – the understanding of others' minds – to the evolution of culture. Tomasello et al. conceive of the accumulation of culture as the product of cultural learning, a kind of learning dependent upon recognizing others' intentionality. They distinguish three levels of this recognition: of intention (what isxtrying to do), of beliefs (what doesxthink aboutp), and of beliefs about beliefs (what doesxthinkythinks aboutp). They then tie these levels to three discrete forms (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Agents, intentions and enculturated apes.Juan Carlos Gómez - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):520-521.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Imitation, cultural learning and the origins of “theory of mind”.Alison Gopnik & Andrew Meltzoff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):521-523.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Child development and theories of culture: A historical perspective.Robin L. Harwood - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):523-523.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cultural learning: Are there functional consequences?Marc D. Mauser - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):524-524.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • A developmental theory requires developmental data.Kim A. Bard - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):511-512.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cultural learning.Michael Tomasello, Ann Cale Kruger & Hilary Horn Ratner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):495-511.
    This target article presents a theory of human cultural learning. Cultural learning is identified with those instances of social learning in which intersubjectivity or perspective-taking plays a vital role, both in the original learning process and in the resulting cognitive product. Cultural learning manifests itself in three forms during human ontogeny: imitative learning, instructed learning, and collaborative learning – in that order. Evidence is provided that this progression arises from the developmental ordering of the underlying social-cognitive concepts and processes involved. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   370 citations  
  • Intentional relations and social understanding.John Barresi & Chris Moore - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):107-122.
    Organisms engage in various activities that are directed at objects, whether real or imagined. Such activities may be termed “intentional relations.” We present a four-level framework of social understanding that organizes the ways in which social organisms represent the intentional relations of themselves and other agents. We presuppose that the information available to an organism about its own intentional relations (or first person information) is qualitatively different from the information available to that organism about other agents’ intentional relations (or third (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   74 citations  
  • Imagination and imitation: Input, acid test, or alchemy?C. M. Heyes - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):131-132.
    Immediate imitation is likely to be a major, direct input to Barresi & Moore's level 2 competence, but deferred imitation is unlikely to play a key role in the transition to level 3, because (1) the attribution of first person knowledge is neither a necessary cause nor an obvious consequence of deferred imitation, and (2) deferred imitation does not correlate phylogenetically with capacities that more plausibly either yield or reflect a concept of intentional agency.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Understanding that looking causes knowing.David R. Olson & Bruce Homer - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):135-135.
    Barresi & Moore provide an impressive account of how the coordination of first and third person information about the self and other could produce an account of intentional relations. They are less explicit as to how the child comes to understand the basic epistemic relation between experience and knowledge, that is, how informational access causes belief. We suggest one route.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Evolution of Primate Cognition.Richard W. Byrne - 2000 - Cognitive Science 24 (3):543-570.
    Comparative analysis of the behavior of modern primates, in conjunction with an accurate phylogenetic tree of relatedness, has the power to chart the early history of human cognitive evolution. Adaptive cognitive changes along this path occurred, it is believed, in response to various forms of complexity; to some extent, theories that relate particular challenges to cognitive adaptations can also be tested against comparative data on primate ecology and behavior. This paper explains the procedures by which data are employed, and uses (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Précis of Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition.Merlin Donald - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):737-748.
    This bold and brilliant book asks the ultimate question of the life sciences: How did the human mind acquire its incomparable power? In seeking the answer, Merlin Donald traces the evolution of human culture and cognition from primitive apes to the era of artificial intelligence, and presents an original theory of how the human mind evolved from its presymbolic form. In the emergence of modern human culture, Donald proposes, there were three radical transitions. During the first, our bipedal but still (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   362 citations  
  • The Ontogenesis of Trust.Fabrice Clément, Melissa Koenig & Paul Harris - 2004 - Mind and Language 19 (4):360-379.
    Psychologists have emphasized children's acquisition of information through firsthand observation. However, many beliefs are acquired from others' testimony. In two experiments, most 4yearolds displayed sceptical trust in testimony. Having heard informants' accurate or inaccurate testimony, they anticipated that informants would continue to display such differential accuracy and they trusted the hitherto reliable informant. Yet they ignored the testimony of the reliable informant if it conflicted with what they themselves had seen. By contrast, threeyearolds were less selective in trusting a reliable (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Animal beliefs and their contents.Frank Dreckmann - 1999 - Erkenntnis 51 (1):597-615.
    This paper investigates whether, or not, the behavior of animals without speech can manifest beliefs and desires. Criteria for the attribution of such beliefs and desires are worked out with reference to Jonathan Bennett's theory of cognitive teleology: A particular ability for learning justifies attributing such beliefs and desires. The conceptual analysis is illustrated by examinations of cognitive ethology and considers higher-order intentionality. It is argued that the behavioral evidence only supports the attribution of first order beliefs and that languageless (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The Biology and Evolution of the Three Psychological Tendencies to Anthropomorphize Biology and Evolution.Marco Antonio Correa Varella - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:400069.
    At the core of anthropomorphism lies a false-positive cognitive bias to over-attribute the pattern of the human body and/or mind. Anthropomorphism is independently discussed in various disciplines, is presumed to have deep biological roots, but its cognitive bases are rarely explored in an integrative way. I present an inclusive, multifaceted interdisciplinary approach to refine the psychological bases of mental anthropomorphism. I have integrated 13 conceptual dissections of folk finalistic reasoning into four psychological inference systems (physical, design, basic-goal and belief stances); (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Perspective taking and belief attribution : from Piaget's theory to children's theory of mind.Pierre Mounoud - unknown
    This paper analyzes the origins and specificity of the recent research trend on the development in children of a Theory of mind which has undergone an impressive expansion over past the fifteen years. A comparison with Piaget's approach is proposed regarding the experimental data available on the coordination of perspectives as well as the epistemological foundations. The issues of the naturalization of the mind and its irreducibility are addressed within the framework of recent reductionist theories advanced by the philosophers of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • In this best of all possible monkey worlds?Harold Gouzoules - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):158-159.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “How monkeys see the world.” Why monkeys?A. H. Harcourt - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):160-161.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Are monkeys nomothetic or idiographic?Linda Mealey - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):161-161.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   48 citations  
  • The modern mind: Its missing parts?R. I. M. Dunbar - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):758-759.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • Human enculturation, chimpanzee enculturation (?) and the nature of imitation.Andrew Whiten - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):538-539.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Imitation without perspective-taking.C. M. Heyes - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):524-525.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • (1 other version)Do we “acquire” culture or vice versa?Jerome Bruner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):515-516.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Introduction: intersubjectivity and empathy.Rasmus Thybo Jensen & Dermot Moran - 2012 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 11 (2):125-133.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Modality and theory of mind: Perspectives from language development and autism.Anna Papafragou - unknown
    It is widely assumed in the developmental literature that certain classes of modal expression appear later in language acquisition than others; specifically, epistemic interpretations lag behind non-epistemic interpretations. An explanation for these findings is proposed in terms of the child’s developing theory of mind, i.e. the ability to attribute to oneself and others mental representations, and to reason inferentially about them. It is hypothesized that epistemic modality crucially implicates theory-of-mind abilities and is therefore expected to depend on prior developments in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Mindreading, mindshaping, and evolution.Matteo Mameli - 2001 - Biology and Philosophy 16 (5):595-626.
    I present and apply some powerful tools for studying human evolution and the impact of cultural resources on it. The tools in question are a theory of niche construction and a theory about the evolutionary significance of extragenetic (and, in particular, of psychological and social) inheritance. These tools are used to show how culturally transmitted resources can be recruited by development and become generatively entrenched. The case study is constituted by those culturally transmitted items that social psychologists call ‘expectancies’. Expectancy (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   56 citations  
  • Developing mental abilities by representing intentionality.Radu J. Bogdan - 2001 - Synthese 129 (2):233-258.
    Communication by shared meaning, themastery of word semantics,metarepresentation and metamentation aremental abilities, uniquely human, that share a sense ofintentionality or reference. The latteris developed by a naive psychology or interpretation – acompetence dedicated to representingintentional relations between conspecifics and the world. Theidea that interpretation builds new mentalabilities around a sense of reference is based on three linesof analysis – conceptual, psychological andevolutionary. The conceptual analysis reveals that a senseof reference is at the heart of the abilitiesin question. Psychological data track (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Lessons from evolution for artificial intelligence?Rudi Lutz - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):766-766.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A natural history of the mind: A guide for cognitive science.Thomas L. Clarke - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):754-755.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Putting cognitive carts before linguistic horses.Derek Bickerton - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):749-750.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Kinesthetic-visual matching, perspective-taking and reflective self-awareness in cultural learning.Robert W. Mitchell - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):530-531.
    Tomasello, Kruger & Ratner deserve congratulations for their well-reasoned ideas on the development of cultural learning. Their arguments are generally convincing, perhaps because their distinctions and developmental relations among types of cultural learning and agency mirror concepts of my own.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What neurodevelopmental disorders can reveal about cognitive architecture.Helen Tager-Flusberg - 2005 - In Peter Carruthers, Stephen Laurence & Stephen P. Stich (eds.), The Innate Mind: Structure and Contents. New York, US: Oxford University Press on Demand. pp. 272--288.
    This chapter begins with an overview of the controversy surrounding the study of children and adults with neurodevelopmental disorders, and how these inform theories of neurocognitive architecture. It weighs the arguments for and against what we might learn from studying individuals who have fundamental biological impairments. It then discusses the example of research on theory of mind in two different disorders — autism and Williams syndrome — which has highlighted a number of important aspects of how this core cognitive capacity (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Perception theory and the attribution of mental states.Philip A. Glotzbach - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):157-158.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Stages versus continuity.Christopher Wills - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):773-773.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From mimetic to mythic culture: Stimulus equivalence effects and prelinguistic cognition.P. J. Hampson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):763-763.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Correct data base: Wrong model?Alexander Marshack - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):767-768.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark