Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The origins of probabilistic inference in human infants.Stephanie Denison & Fei Xu - 2014 - Cognition 130 (3):335-347.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • The number sense represents (rational) numbers.Sam Clarke & Jacob Beck - 2021 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 44:1-57.
    On a now orthodox view, humans and many other animals possess a “number sense,” or approximate number system, that represents number. Recently, this orthodox view has been subject to numerous critiques that question whether the ANS genuinely represents number. We distinguish three lines of critique – the arguments from congruency, confounds, and imprecision – and show that none succeed. We then provide positive reasons to think that the ANS genuinely represents numbers, and not just non-numerical confounds or exotic substitutes for (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Tracking Multiple Statistics: Simultaneous Learning of Object Names and Categories in English and Mandarin Speakers.Chi-Hsin Chen, Lisa Gershkoff-Stowe, Chih-Yi Wu, Hintat Cheung & Chen Yu - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (6):1485-1509.
    Two experiments were conducted to examine adult learners' ability to extract multiple statistics in simultaneously presented visual and auditory input. Experiment 1 used a cross‐situational learning paradigm to test whether English speakers were able to use co‐occurrences to learn word‐to‐object mappings and concurrently form object categories based on the commonalities across training stimuli. Experiment 2 replicated the first experiment and further examined whether speakers of Mandarin, a language in which final syllables of object names are more predictive of category membership (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Statistics is Founded on Entropy, Not Evolutionary Psychology.Fred L. Bookstein - 2014 - Biological Theory 9 (1):108-112.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Tychomancy: Inferring Probability from Causal Structure, by Michael Strevens: Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2013, pp. xiv + 265, US$39.95. [REVIEW]Michael Bishop - 2014 - Australasian Journal of Philosophy 92 (4):808-811.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Macphail’s Null Hypothesis of Vertebrate Intelligence: Insights From Avian Cognition.Amalia P. M. Bastos & Alex H. Taylor - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Equidynamics and reliable reasoning about frequencies: Michael Strevens: Tychomancy: Inferring probability from causal structure. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 265pp, $39.95 HB.Marshall Abrams, Frederick Eberhardt & Michael Strevens - 2015 - Metascience 24 (2):173-188.
    A symposium on Michael Strevens' book "Tychomancy", concerning the psychological roots and historical significance of physical intuition about probability in physics, biology, and elsewhere.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Invisible Hand: Toddlers Connect Probabilistic Events With Agentive Causes.Yang Wu, Paul Muentener & Laura E. Schulz - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (8):1854-1876.
    Children posit unobserved causes when events appear to occur spontaneously. What about when events appear to occur probabilistically? Here toddlers saw arbitrary causal relationships in a fixed, alternating order. The relationships were then changed in one of two ways. In the Deterministic condition, the event order changed ; in the Probabilistic condition, the causal relationships changed. As intended, toddlers looked equally long at both changes. We then introduced a previously unseen candidate cause. Toddlers looked longer at the appearance of a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Integrating Physical Constraints in Statistical Inference by 11-Month-Old Infants.Stephanie Denison & Fei Xu - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (5):885-908.
    Much research on cognitive development focuses either on early-emerging domain-specific knowledge or domain-general learning mechanisms. However, little research examines how these sources of knowledge interact. Previous research suggests that young infants can make inferences from samples to populations (Xu & Garcia, 2008) and 11- to 12.5-month-old infants can integrate psychological and physical knowledge in probabilistic reasoning (Teglas, Girotto, Gonzalez, & Bonatti, 2007; Xu & Denison, 2009). Here, we ask whether infants can integrate a physical constraint of immobility into a statistical (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Expectancy violations promote learning in young children.Aimee E. Stahl & Lisa Feigenson - 2017 - Cognition 163 (C):1-14.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • The origins of inquiry: inductive inference and exploration in early childhood.Laura Schulz - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (7):382-389.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • A description–experience gap in statistical intuitions: Of smart babies, risk-savvy chimps, intuitive statisticians, and stupid grown-ups.Christin Schulze & Ralph Hertwig - 2021 - Cognition 210 (C):104580.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Role of Within-Category Variability in Category-Based Induction: A Developmental Study.Marjorie Rhodes & Daniel Brickman - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (8):1561-1573.
    The present studies tested the hypothesis that strong assumptions about within-category homogeneity impede children’s recognition of the inductive value of diverse samples of evidence. In Study 1a, children (7-year-olds) and adults were randomly assigned to receive a prime emphasizing within-category variability, a prime emphasizing within-category similarities, or to not receive a prime. Only following the variability prime, children demonstrated a reliable preference for evaluating diverse over nondiverse samples to determine whether there is support for a category-wide generalization. Adults demonstrated a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The case for moral empiricism.Shaun Nichols - 2021 - Analysis 81 (3):549-567.
    It is an old and venerable idea in philosophy that morality is built into us, and this nativist view has seen a resurgence of late. Indeed, the prevailing systematic account of how we acquire complex moral representations is a nativist view inspired by arguments in Chomskyan linguistics. In this article, I review the leading argument for moral nativism – the poverty of the moral stimulus. I defend a systematic empiricist alternative that draws on the resources of statistical learning. Such an (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Young children’s use of statistical sampling evidence to infer the subjectivity of preferences.Lili Ma & Fei Xu - 2011 - Cognition 120 (3):403-411.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • The developmental and cultural psychology of free will.Tamar Kushnir - 2018 - Philosophy Compass 13 (11):e12529.
    This paper provides an account of the developmental origins of our belief in free will based on research from a range of ages—infants, preschoolers, older children, and adults—and across cultures. The foundations of free will beliefs are in infants' understanding of intentional action—their ability to use context to infer when agents are free to “do otherwise” and when they are constrained. In early childhood, new knowledge about causes of action leads to new abilities to imagine constraints on action. Moreover, unlike (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • How Young Children Learn From Examples: Descriptive and Inferential Problems.Charles W. Kalish, Sunae Kim & Andrew G. Young - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (8):1427-1448.
    Three experiments with preschool- and young school-aged children (N = 75 and 53) explored the kinds of relations children detect in samples of instances (descriptive problem) and how they generalize those relations to new instances (inferential problem). Each experiment initially presented a perfect biconditional relation between two features (e.g., all and only frogs are blue). Additional examples undermined one of the component conditional relations (not all frogs are blue) but supported another (only frogs are blue). Preschool-aged children did not distinguish (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • How children use examples to make conditional predictions.Charles W. Kalish - 2010 - Cognition 116 (1):1-14.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Young children do not succeed in choice tasks that imply evaluating chances.Vittorio Girotto, Laura Fontanari, Michel Gonzalez, Giorgio Vallortigara & Agnès Blaye - 2016 - Cognition 152 (C):32-39.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Verbal framing of statistical evidence drives children’s preference inferences.Laura E. Garvin & Amanda L. Woodward - 2015 - Cognition 138 (C):35-48.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Throwing out the Bayesian baby with the optimal bathwater: Response to Endress.Michael C. Frank - 2013 - Cognition 128 (3):417-423.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • The Feasibility of Folk Science.Frank C. Keil - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (5):826-862.
    If folk science means individuals having well worked out mechanistic theories of the workings of the world, then it is not feasible. Laypeople’s explanatory understandings are remarkably coarse, full of gaps, and often full of inconsistencies. Even worse, most people overestimate their own understandings. Yet recent views suggest that formal scientists may not be so different. In spite of these limitations, science somehow works and its success offers hope for the feasibility of folk science as well. The success of science (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Testimony and observation of statistical evidence interact in adults' and children's category-based induction.Zoe Finiasz, Susan A. Gelman & Tamar Kushnir - 2024 - Cognition 244 (C):105707.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Violations of Core Knowledge Shape Early Learning.Aimee E. Stahl & Lisa Feigenson - 2019 - Topics in Cognitive Science 11 (1):136-153.
    This paper discusses recent evidence that violations of core knowledge offer special learning opportunities for infants and young children. Children make predictions about the world from the youngest ages. When their fail to match observed data, they show an enhanced drive to seek and retain new information about entities that violated their expectations. Finally, the authors draw comparisons between children and adults, and with other species, to explore how surprise shapes thought more broadly.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Intuitive statistical inferences in chimpanzees and humans follow Weber’s law.Johanna Eckert, Josep Call, Jonas Hermes, Esther Herrmann & Hannes Rakoczy - 2018 - Cognition 180 (C):99-107.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The Development of Spatial–Temporal, Probability, and Covariation Information to Infer Continuous Causal Processes.Selma Dündar-Coecke, Andrew Tolmie & Anne Schlottmann - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    This paper considers how 5- to 11-year-olds’ verbal reasoning about the causality underlying extended, dynamic natural processes links to various facets of their statistical thinking. Such continuous processes typically do not provide perceptually distinct causes and effect, and previous work suggests that spatial–temporal analysis, the ability to analyze spatial configurations that change over time, is a crucial predictor of reasoning about causal mechanism in such situations. Work in the Humean tradition to causality has long emphasized on the importance of statistical (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Time course of visual attention in statistical learning of words and categories.Chi-Hsin Chen, Chen Yu, Damian Fricker, Thomas G. Smith & Lisa Gershkoff-Stowe - 2010 - In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Non-symbolic halving in an amazonian indigene group.Koleen McCrink, Elizabeth Spelke, Stanislas Dehaene & Pierre Pica - 2013 - Developmental Science 16 (3):451-462.
    Much research supports the existence of an Approximate Number System (ANS) that is recruited by infants, children, adults, and non-human animals to generate coarse, non-symbolic representations of number. This system supports simple arithmetic operations such as addition, subtraction, and ordering of amounts. The current study tests whether an intuition of a more complex calculation, division, exists in an indigene group in the Amazon, the Mundurucu, whose language includes no words for large numbers. Mundurucu children were presented with a video event (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations