Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Reducing Ethical Hazards in Knowledge Production.Alan Cottey - 2016 - Science and Engineering Ethics 22 (2):367-389.
    This article discusses the ethics of knowledge production from a cultural point of view, in contrast with the more usual emphasis on the ethical issues facing individuals involved in KP. Here, the emphasis is on the cultural environment within which individuals, groups and institutions perform KP. A principal purpose is to suggest ways in which reliable scientific knowledge could be produced more efficiently. The distinction between ethical hazard and ethical behaviour is noted. Ethical hazards cannot be eliminated but they can (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Speaking Power to Truth: Digital Discourse and the Public Intellectual.Michael Keren & Richard Hawkins (eds.) - 2015 - Athabasca University Press‎.
    Online discourse has created a new media environment for contributions to public life, one that challenges the social significance of the role of public intellectuals—intellectuals who, whether by choice or by circumstance, offer commentary on issues of the day. The value of such commentary is rooted in the assumption that, by virtue of their training and experience, intellectuals possess knowledge—that they understand what constitutes knowledge with respect to a particular topic, are able to distinguish it from mere opinion, and are (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Experts within Democracy: The Turner Version.Joseph Agassi - 2015 - Philosophy of the Social Sciences 45 (3):370-384.
    Stephen Turner defends the sociopolitical role that experts—mainly but not only of the scientific kind—play in modern democratic society and explores means for increasing the rationality of their employment. Laudable though this is, at times Turner goes into more detail than democratic principles require; in his enthusiasm for rationality, he aims at levels of adequacy that are not always within the grasp of democracy.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Notes on the cultural significance of the sciences.Wallis A. Suchting - 1994 - Science & Education 3 (1):1-56.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Sex differences in sexuality: what is their relevance to sex roles?Shirley Weitz - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):202-202.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Beyond the sociobiology of sexuality: predictive hypotheses.John Alcock - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):181-182.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Male and female choice in human sexuality.Diane McGuinness - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):194-195.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Konrad Lorenz and Humpty Dumpty: some ethology for Donald Symons.Mark Ridley - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):196-196.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The clarification of proximate mechanisms.Dorothy Tennov - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):200-200.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Computation, consciousness and cognition.George A. Miller - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):146-146.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Psychology and computational architecture.John Haugeland - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):138-139.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Emotions Beyond Regulation: Backgrounded Emotions in Science and Trust.Jack Barbalet - 2011 - Emotion Review 3 (1):36-43.
    Emotions are understood sociologically as experiences of involvement. Emotion regulation influences the type, incidence, and expression of emotions. Regulation occurs through physical processes prior to an emotions episode, through social interaction in which a person’s emotions are modified due to the reactions of others to them, and by a person’s self-modification or management of emotions which they are consciously aware of. This article goes on to show that there are emotions which the emoting subject is not consciously aware of. Therefore, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Normal Accidents of Expertise.Stephen P. Turner - 2010 - Minerva 48 (3):239-258.
    Charles Perrow used the term normal accidents to characterize a type of catastrophic failure that resulted when complex, tightly coupled production systems encountered a certain kind of anomalous event. These were events in which systems failures interacted with one another in a way that could not be anticipated, and could not be easily understood and corrected. Systems of the production of expert knowledge are increasingly becoming tightly coupled. Unlike classical science, which operated with a long time horizon, many current forms (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Geographie des Wissens und der Wissenschaften: Von der Encyclopédie zur Konstitutionstheorie.Thomas Mormann - 2005 - In Elisabeth Nemeth & Nicolas Roudet (eds.), Paris – Wien: Enzyklopädien im Vergleich. Springer.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Some remarks on the re-building of the category of essence and the reflective modernity.Zhen Han - 2010 - Frontiers of Philosophy in China 5 (1):134-141.
    If modernity is manifested as essentialism, postmodernity is manifested as anti-essentialism. Modernity is, in essence, human beings’ discovery of their own power, and is based on rational knowledge that has grasped the essence of things. In fact, in the discourse system of modernity, the various concepts of “essence” connote nothing but people’s imaginative constructions and rational conjectures about objects. In the past, our order, be it internal or external, was in essence guaranteed by God. Afterwards, all “essences”, as essences, must (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Computation and cognition: Issues in the foundation of cognitive science.Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):111-32.
    The computational view of mind rests on certain intuitions regarding the fundamental similarity between computation and cognition. We examine some of these intuitions and suggest that they derive from the fact that computers and human organisms are both physical systems whose behavior is correctly described as being governed by rules acting on symbolic representations. Some of the implications of this view are discussed. It is suggested that a fundamental hypothesis of this approach is that there is a natural domain of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   669 citations  
  • Reading fleck : Questions on philosophy and science.Eva Hedfors - 2006 - Dissertation, Royal Institute of Technology, Stockholm
    The present thesis is based on a scientifically-informed, contextualized and historicized reading of Ludwik Fleck. In addition to his monograph, the material studied includes his additional philosophical writings, his internationally-published scientific articles and two, thus-far-unstudied postwar Polish papers related to his Buchenwald experiences. The sources provided by Fleck have been traced back to the time of their origin. Based on the above material, it is argued that, rather than relativizing science and deeply influencing Kuhn, Fleck, attempting to participate in the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Public Understanding of Science—A Rhetorical Invention.Simon Locke - 2002 - Science, Technology, and Human Values 27 (1):87-111.
    This article contributes to the development of a rhetorical approach to the public understanding of science or science literacy. It is argued that rhetoric promises an alternative approach to deficit models that treat people as faulty scientists. Some tensions in the relevant rhetorical literature need resolution. These center on the application to science of an Aristotelian conception of rhetorical reasoning as enthymematic, without breaking from the Platonic/aristotelian division between technical and public spheres. The former opens science to the potential of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Cryptozoology and the Troubles with “Skeptics” and Mainstream Pundits.Henry Bauer - 2014 - Journal of Scientific Exploration 27 (4).
    Abominable Science by Daniel Loxton and Donald R. Prothero. Columbia University Press, 2013. xvi + 411 pp. $29.95 (hardcover). ISBN 978-0231153201. This book is superbly produced by a prominent university press. It is also intellectually shoddy, even dishonest. Science is described in naïve shibboleths that bear no relation to how science is actually done. The chapters about individual cryptids are chock-full of misrepresentation and evasion of the best evidence. Abominable Science is unsatisfactory in ways that are all too common with (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Role of Evidence in Chronic Care Decision-Making.Fabrizio Macagno & Sarah Bigi - 2020 - Topoi 40 (2):343-358.
    In the domain of medical science, factual evidence is usually considered as the criterion on which to base decisions and construct hypotheses. Evidence-based medicine is the translation of this approach into the field of patient care, and it means providing only the type of care that is based on evidence that proves its effectiveness and appropriateness. However, while the literature has focused on the types and force of evidence used to establish the recommendation and treatment guidelines, the problem of how (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Evolutionary causation: how proximate is ultimate?Richard E. Whalen - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):202-203.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Forcible rape and human sexuality.Ted L. Huston & Gilbert Geis - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):186-187.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sex differences and intent.G. Mitchell - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):195-196.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Précis of The evolution of human sexuality.Donald Symons - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):171-181.
    Patterns in the data on human sexuality support the hypothesis that the bases of sexual emotions are products of natural selection. Most generally, the universal existence of laws, rules, and gossip about sex, the pervasive interest in other people's sex lives, the widespread seeking of privacy for sexual intercourse, and the secrecy that normally permeates sexual conduct imply a history of reproductive competition. More specifically, the typical differences between men and women in sexual feelings can be explained most parsimoniously as (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   113 citations  
  • Computation without representation.Stephen P. Stich - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):152-152.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Cognition is not computation, for the reasons that computers don't solve the mind-body problems.Walter B. Weimer - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):152-153.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Functional architectures for cognition: are simple inferences possible?Steven W. Zucker - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):153-154.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Explanations in theories of language and of imagery.Steven Pinker - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):147-148.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The elusive visual processing mode: Implications of the architecture/algorithm distinction.Roberta L. Klatzky - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):142-143.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • In defence of the armchair.Michael Fortescue - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):135-136.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Computation, cognition, and representation.John Hell - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):139-139.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Computers and knowledge: a dialogical approach. [REVIEW]Christian Brassac - 2006 - AI and Society 20 (3):249-270.
    Artificial intelligence researchers interested in knowledge and in designing and implementing digitized artifacts for representing or sharing knowledge play a crucial role in the development of a knowledge-based economy. They help answer the question of how the computer devices they develop can be appropriated by the collectives that manage the flow of knowledge and the know-how underlying human organizations. A dialogical, constructivist view of interaction processes permits theorizing the role of digital tools, seen as sociotechnical devices that serve both as (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Reverence and Ethics in Science.Jeffrey Kovac - 2013 - Science and Engineering Ethics 19 (3):745-756.
    Codes of ethics abound in science, but the question of why such codes should be obeyed is rarely asked. Various reasons for obeying a professional code have been proposed, but all are unsatisfactory in that they do not really motivate behavior. This article suggests that the long forgotten virtue of reverence provides both a reason to obey a professional code and motivation to do so. In addition, it discusses the importance of reverence as a cardinal virtue for scientists drawing on (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the Organism-Environment Distinction in Psychology.Daniel K. Palmer - 2004 - Behavior and Philosophy 32 (2):317 - 347.
    Most psychology begins with a distinction between organism and environment, where the two are implicitly (and sometimes explicitly) conceptualized as flipsides of a skin-severed space. This paper examines that conceptualization. Dewey and Bentley's (1949) account of firm naming is used to show that psychologists have, in general, (1) employed the skin as a morphological criterion for distinguishing organisms from backgrounds, and (2) equated background with environment. This two-step procedure, which in this article is named the morphological conception of organism, is (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Going Public: Good Scientific Conduct.Gitte Meyer & Peter Sandøe - 2012 - Science and Engineering Ethics 18 (2):173-197.
    The paper addresses issues of scientific conduct regarding relations between science and the media, relations between scientists and journalists, and attitudes towards the public at large. In the large and increasing body of literature on scientific conduct and misconduct, these issues seem underexposed as ethical challenges. Consequently, individual scientists here tend to be left alone with problems and dilemmas, with no guidance for good conduct. Ideas are presented about how to make up for this omission. Using a practical, ethical approach, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Rationalism and the “rational actor assumption” in realist international relations theory.Colin Wight & Brian C. Schmidt - 2023 - Journal of International Political Theory 19 (2):158-182.
    The commitment to the rational actor model of state behavior is said to be a core assumption of realist theory. This assumption is listed in most textbook accounts of realism. Yet is rationality a core supposition of realist theory, and if so, what kind of rationality is implied in these claims? Debate on the relationship between realism, and what is often labeled as rationality is replete with misunderstandings. Authors deploy terms such as rationality, rationalism, and rational actor in diverse and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Popper: Critical Rationalist, Conventionalist, and Virtue Epistemologist.Patrick M. Duerr - 2023 - Hopos: The Journal of the International Society for the History of Philosophy of Science 13 (1):54-90.
    This article revisits Karl Popper’s falsificationist methodology with respect to three tasks. The first is to illuminate and systematize Popper’s methodological views in light of his core epistemological commitments. A second and related objective is to gauge which aspects of falsificationism should be identified as “conventionalist”—a label that Popper himself uses (albeit with qualifications) but that is compromised by and, thus, stands in need of elucidation because of Popper’s idiosyncratic understanding of conventionalism. Third, by elaborating Popper’s virtue-epistemological, dialogical model of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Authority of the Scientific Rejection of Pseudo-Science.R. G. A. Dolby - 1989 - Bulletin of Science, Technology and Society 9 (3):283-293.
    The following three papers (by R.G.A. Dolby, R.N.D. Martin, and A. Thomson) were presented to the Science & Religion Forum at its annual meeting in Liverpool, Britain, in March 1988, under the general theme of Tradition & Authority in Science and Religion. An abbreviated version of the fourth paper given at that meeting re-entitled STS Perspective: Value Systems, by W.F. Williams, is printed on pp. 219-221 of this volume (Vol. 9, No. 4). A copy of the full version is available (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • An Evaluation of the Belief in Science Scale.Neil Dagnall, Andrew Denovan, Kenneth Graham Drinkwater & Andrew Parker - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Selecting for a sociobiological fit.Julia R. Heiman - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):189-190.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • An interactionist perspective on human sexuality.Mark R. Hoffman - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):190-191.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pair bonding and proximal mechanisms.Glenn E. King - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):191-192.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is science sexist?Michael Ruse - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):197-198.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The evolution of human sexuality revisited.Donald Symons - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):203-214.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Female sexual adaptability: a consequence of the absence of natural selection among females.J. Richard Udry - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):201-202.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The biosocial evolution of human sexuality.Milton Diamond - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):184-186.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive representation and the process-architecture distinction.Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):154-169.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   76 citations  
  • Computation and symbolization.William E. Smythe - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):151-152.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Functional architecture and model validation.Martin Ringle - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):150-151.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive penetrability: let us not forget about memory.James R. Miller - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):146-146.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark