Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. On metaphoric representation.Gregory L. Murphy - 1996 - Cognition 60 (2):173-204.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   65 citations  
  • A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity.Vincent Walsh - 2003 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 (11):483-488.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   192 citations  
  • Metaphors We Live by.Max Black - 1980 - Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 40 (2):208-210.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   703 citations  
  • Metaphors We Live By.George Lakoff & Mark Johnson - 1980 - Ethics 93 (3):619-621.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1373 citations  
  • Language and memory for object location.Harmen B. Gudde, Kenny R. Coventry & Paul E. Engelhardt - 2016 - Cognition 153 (C):99-107.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Space–time interdependence: Evidence against asymmetric mapping between time and space.Zhenguang G. Cai & Louise Connell - 2015 - Cognition 136 (C):268-281.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Close to me: Multisensory space representations for action and pre-reflexive consciousness of oneself-in-the-world.Dorothée Legrand, Claudio Brozzoli, Yves Rossetti & Alessandro Farnè - 2007 - Consciousness and Cognition 16 (3):687-699.
    Philosophical considerations as well as several recent studies from neurophysiology, neuropsychology, and psychophysics converged in showing that the peripersonal space is structured in a body-centred manner and represented through integrated sensory inputs. Multisensory representations may deserve the function of coding peripersonal space for avoiding or interacting with objects. Neuropsychological evidence is reviewed for dynamic interactions between space representations and action execution, as revealed by the behavioural effects that the use of a tool, as a physical extension of the reachable space, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Time in the mind: Using space to think about time.Daniel Casasanto & Lera Boroditsky - 2008 - Cognition 106 (2):579-593.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   120 citations  
  • Demonstratives, joint attention, and the emergence of grammar.Holger Diessel - 2006 - Cognitive Linguistics 17 (4):463-489.
    Drawing on recent work in developmental and comparative psychology, this paper argues that demonstratives function to coordinate the interlocutors' joint focus of attention, which is one of the most basic functions of human communication. The communicative importance of demonstratives is reflected in a number of properties that together characterize them as a particular word class: In contrast to other closed-class expressions, demonstratives are universal, they are generally so old that their roots cannot be traced back to other linguistic items, they (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Language within your reach: Near–far perceptual space and spatial demonstratives.Kenny R. Coventry, Berenice Valdés, Alejandro Castillo & Pedro Guijarro-Fuentes - 2008 - Cognition 108 (3):889-895.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • A kinematic approach to the conceptual representations of this and that.Claudia Bonfiglioli, Chiara Finocchiaro, Benno Gesierich, Francesco Rositani & Massimo Vescovi - 2009 - Cognition 111 (2):270-274.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Close to me: Multisensory space representations for action and pre-reflexive consciousness of oneself-in-the-world.Dorothee Lang, Claudio Brozzoli, Yves Rossetti & Alessandro Farne - 2007 - Consciousness and Cognition 16 (3):687-699.
    Philosophical considerations as well as several recent studies from neurophysiology, neuropsychology, and psychophysics converged in showing that the peripersonal space is structured in a body-centred manner and represented through integrated sensory inputs. Multisensory representations may deserve the function of coding peripersonal space for avoiding or interacting with objects. Neuropsychological evidence is reviewed for dynamic interactions between space representations and action execution, as revealed by the behavioural effects that the use of a tool, as a physical extension of the reachable space, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Space and Time in the Child’s Mind: Evidence for a Cross-Dimensional Asymmetry.Daniel Casasanto, Olga Fotakopoulou & Lera Boroditsky - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (3):387-405.
    What is the relationship between space and time in the human mind? Studies in adults show an asymmetric relationship between mental representations of these basic dimensions of experience: Representations of time depend on space more than representations of space depend on time. Here we investigated the relationship between space and time in the developing mind. Native Greek‐speaking children watched movies of two animals traveling along parallel paths for different distances or durations and judged the spatial and temporal aspects of these (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • The plasticity of near space: Evidence for contraction.Stella F. Lourenco & Matthew R. Longo - 2009 - Cognition 112 (3):451-456.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations