Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. (1 other version)The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information.George A. Miller - 1956 - Psychological Review 63 (2):81-97.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   925 citations  
  • Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing.Neil Burgess & Graham J. Hitch - 1999 - Psychological Review 106 (3):551-581.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   46 citations  
  • The phonological loop as a language learning device.Alan Baddeley, Susan Gathercole & Costanza Papagno - 1998 - Psychological Review 105 (1):158-173.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   74 citations  
  • The complexities of complex span: explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults.Donna M. Bayliss, Christopher Jarrold, Deborah M. Gunn & Alan D. Baddeley - 2003 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 132 (1):71.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • The Psychology of Intelligence.Rex Knight, Jean Piaget, M. Piercy & D. E. Berlyne - 1951 - Philosophical Quarterly 1 (5):470.
    First Published in 1999. Routledge is an imprint of Taylor & Francis, an informa company.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   109 citations  
  • EPAM‐like Models of Recognition and Learning.Edward A. Feigenbaum & Herbert A. Simon - 1984 - Cognitive Science 8 (4):305-336.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  • Modeling the Developmental Patterning of Finiteness Marking in English, Dutch, German, and Spanish Using MOSAIC.Daniel Freudenthal, Julian M. Pine, Javier Aguado-Orea & Fernand Gobet - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (2):311-341.
    In this study, we apply MOSAIC (model of syntax acquisition in children) to the simulation of the developmental patterning of children's optional infinitive (OI) errors in 4 languages: English, Dutch, German, and Spanish. MOSAIC, which has already simulated this phenomenon in Dutch and English, now implements a learning mechanism that better reflects the theoretical assumptions underlying it, as well as a chunking mechanism that results in frequent phrases being treated as 1 unit. Using 1, identical model that learns from child‐directed (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Modeling the Development of Children's Use of Optional Infinitives in Dutch and English Using MOSAIC.Daniel Freudenthal, Julian M. Pine & Fernand Gobet - 2006 - Cognitive Science 30 (2):277-310.
    In this study we use a computational model of language learning called model of syntax acquisition in children (MOSAIC) to investigate the extent to which the optional infinitive (OI) phenomenon in Dutch and English can be explained in terms of a resource-limited distributional analysis of Dutch and English child-directed speech. The results show that the same version of MOSAIC is able to simulate changes in the pattern of finiteness marking in 2 children learning Dutch and 2 children learning English as (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Five Seconds or Sixty? Presentation Time in Expert Memory.Fernand Gobet & Herbert A. Simon - 2000 - Cognitive Science 24 (4):651-682.
    For many years, the game of chess has provided an invaluable task environment for research on cognition, in particular on the differences between novices and experts and the learning that removes these differences, and upon the structure of human memory and its paramaters. The template theory presented by Gobet and Simon based on the EPAM theory offers precise predictions on cognitive processes during the presentation and recall of chess positions. This article describes the behavior of CHREST, a computer implementation of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • What forms the chunks in a subject's performance? Lessons from the CHREST computational model of learning.Peter C. R. Lane, Fernand Gobet & Peter C.-H. Cheng - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (1):128-129.
    Computational models of learning provide an alternative technique for identifying the number and type of chunks used by a subject in a specific task. Results from applying CHREST to chess expertise support the theoretical framework of Cowan and a limit in visual short-term memory capacity of 3–4 looms. An application to learning from diagrams illustrates different identifiable forms of chunk.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity.Nelson Cowan - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (1):87-114.
    Miller (1956) summarized evidence that people can remember about seven chunks in short-term memory (STM) tasks. However, that number was meant more as a rough estimate and a rhetorical device than as a real capacity limit. Others have since suggested that there is a more precise capacity limit, but that it is only three to five chunks. The present target article brings together a wide variety of data on capacity limits suggesting that the smaller capacity limit is real. Capacity limits (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   402 citations  
  • The primacy model: A new model of immediate serial recall.Michael P. A. Page & Dennis Norris - 1998 - Psychological Review 105 (4):761-781.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   37 citations  
  • Cross-linguistic regularities in the frequency of number words.S. Dehaene - 1992 - Cognition 43 (1):1-29.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • Oscillator-based memory for serial order.Gordon D. A. Brown, Tim Preece & Charles Hulme - 2000 - Psychological Review 107 (1):127-181.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   77 citations