- Learning and applying contextual constraints in sentence comprehension.Mark F. St John & James L. McClelland - 1990 - Artificial Intelligence 46 (1-2):217-257.details
|
|
(1 other version)A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming.Mark S. Seidenberg & James L. McClelland - 1989 - Psychological Review 96 (4):523-568.details
|
|
Structure and Deterioration of Semantic Memory: A Neuropsychological and Computational Investigation.Timothy T. Rogers, Matthew A. Lambon Ralph, Peter Garrard, Sasha Bozeat, James L. McClelland, John R. Hodges & Karalyn Patterson - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (1):205-235.details
|
|
(3 other versions)The Past-Tense Debate The past and future of the past tense.Steven Pinker & Michael T. Ullman - 2002 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (11):456-463.details
|
|
(3 other versions)The past and future of the past tense.Steven Pinker & Michael Ullman - 2002 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (11):456-463.details
|
|
(1 other version)Finding Structure in Time.Jeffrey L. Elman - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (2):179-211.details
|
|
Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains.David C. Plaut, James L. McClelland, Mark S. Seidenberg & Karalyn Patterson - 1996 - Psychological Review 103 (1):56-115.details
|
|
Doing Without Schema Hierarchies: A Recurrent Connectionist Approach to Normal and Impaired Routine Sequential Action.Matthew Botvinick & David C. Plaut - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (2):395-429.details
|
|
The Story Gestalt: A Model Of Knowledge‐Intensive Processes in Text Comprehension.Mark F. John - 1992 - Cognitive Science 16 (2):271-306.details
|
|
Knowing in the context of acting: The task dynamics of the A-not-B error.Linda B. Smith, Esther Thelen, Robert Titzer & Dewey McLin - 1999 - Psychological Review 106 (2):235-260.details
|
|
A connectionist model of a continuous developmental transition in the balance scale task.Anna C. Schapiro & James L. McClelland - 2009 - Cognition 110 (3):395-411.details
|
|
Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: how important is starting small?Douglas L. T. Rohde & David C. Plaut - 1999 - Cognition 72 (1):67-109.details
|
|
(1 other version)First Principles Organize Attention to and Learning About Relevant Data: Number and the Animate‐Inanimate Distinction as Examples.Rochel Gelman - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (1):79-106.details
|
|
Connectionist models of recognition memory: Constraints imposed by learning and forgetting functions.Roger Ratcliff - 1990 - Psychological Review 97 (2):285-308.details
|
|
The Harmonie Mind. From Neural Computation to Optimality-Theoretic Grammar.Paul Smolensky & Géraldine Legendre - 2009 - Journal for General Philosophy of Science / Zeitschrift für Allgemeine Wissenschaftstheorie 40 (1):141-147.details
|
|
Short-term memory for serial order: A recurrent neural network model.Matthew M. Botvinick & David C. Plaut - 2006 - Psychological Review 113 (2):201-233.details
|
|
Familiarity breeds differentiation: A subjective-likelihood approach to the effects of experience in recognition memory.James L. McClelland & Mark Chappell - 1998 - Psychological Review 105 (4):724-760.details
|
|
Learning and connectionist representations.David E. Rumelhart & Peter M. Todd - 1993 - In David E. Meyer & Sylvan Kornblum (eds.), Attention and Performance XIV: Synergies in Experimental Psychology, Artificial Intelligence, and Cognitive Neuroscience. MIT Press. pp. 3--30.details
|
|
A simple model from a powerful framework that spans levels of analysis.Timothy T. Rogers & James L. McClelland - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (6):729-749.details
|
|
Structured statistical models of inductive reasoning.Charles Kemp & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2009 - Psychological Review 116 (1):20-58.details
|
|
Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory.James L. McClelland, Bruce L. McNaughton & Randall C. O'Reilly - 1995 - Psychological Review 102 (3):419-457.details
|
|
A neurocomputational account of taxonomic responding and fast mapping in early word learning.Julien Mayor & Kim Plunkett - 2010 - Psychological Review 117 (1):1-31.details
|
|
Constraints on knowledge and cognitive development.Frank C. Keil - 1981 - Psychological Review 88 (3):197-227.details
|
|
From the Lexicon to Expectations About Kinds: A Role for Associative Learning.Eliana Colunga & Linda B. Smith - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (2):347-382.details
|
|
When should we expect indirect effects in human contingency learning.D. Sternberg & James L. McClelland - 2009 - In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. pp. 206--211.details
|
|
The dynamics of embodiment: A field theory of infant perseverative reaching.Esther Thelen, Gregor Schöner, Christian Scheier & Linda B. Smith - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (1):1-34.details
|
|
Categories and induction in young children.Susan A. Gelman & Ellen M. Markman - 1986 - Cognition 23 (3):183-209.details
|
|
Learning Continuous Probability Distributions with Symmetric Diffusion Networks.Javier R. Movellan & James L. McClelland - 1993 - Cognitive Science 17 (4):463-496.details
|
|
The influence of categories on perception: Explaining the perceptual magnet effect as optimal statistical inference.Naomi H. Feldman, Thomas L. Griffiths & James L. Morgan - 2009 - Psychological Review 116 (4):752-782.details
|
|