Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. (1 other version)Core knowledge.Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2000 - American Psychologist 55 (11):1233-1243.
    Complex cognitive skills such as reading and calculation and complex cognitive achievements such as formal science and mathematics may depend on a set of building block systems that emerge early in human ontogeny and phylogeny. These core knowledge systems show characteristic limits of domain and task specificity: Each serves to represent a particular class of entities for a particular set of purposes. By combining representations from these systems, however human cognition may achieve extraordinary flexibility. Studies of cognition in human infants (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   213 citations  
  • The origin of concepts.Susan Carey - 2009 - New York: Oxford University Press.
    Only human beings have a rich conceptual repertoire with concepts like tort, entropy, Abelian group, mannerism, icon and deconstruction. How have humans constructed these concepts? And once they have been constructed by adults, how do children acquire them? While primarily focusing on the second question, in The Origin of Concepts , Susan Carey shows that the answers to both overlap substantially. Carey begins by characterizing the innate starting point for conceptual development, namely systems of core cognition. Representations of core cognition (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   480 citations  
  • Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins.Louis M. Herman, Douglas G. Richards & James P. Wolz - 1984 - Cognition 16 (2):129-219.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   143 citations  
  • Varieties of numerical abilities.Stanislas Dehaene - 1992 - Cognition 44 (1-2):1-42.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   116 citations  
  • Brightness and size transposition by rhesus monkeys.Robert Pasnak & Steven L. Kurtz - 1987 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 25 (2):109-112.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Children's understanding of counting.Karen Wynn - 1990 - Cognition 36 (2):155-193.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   143 citations  
  • Grey parrot number acquisition: The inference of cardinal value from ordinal position on the numeral list.Irene M. Pepperberg & Susan Carey - 2012 - Cognition 125 (2):219-232.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in pirahã: Another look at the D e sign features} of human L anguage.Daniel L. Everett - 2005 - Current Anthropology 46 (4):621--646.
    The Pirahã language challenges simplistic application of Hockett’s nearly universally accepted design features of human language by showing that some of these features may be culturally constrained. In particular, Pirahã culture constrains communication to nonabstract subjects which fall within the immediate experience of interlocutors. This constraint explains a number of very surprising features of Pirahã grammar and culture: the absence of numbers of any kind or a concept of counting and of any terms for quantification, the absence of color terms, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   110 citations  
  • (1 other version)Core knowledge.Elizabeth S. Spelke & Katherine D. Kinzler - 2007 - Developmental Science 10 (1):89-96.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   147 citations  
  • Number as a cognitive technology: Evidence from Pirahã language and cognition.Michael C. Frank, Daniel L. Everett, Evelina Fedorenko & Edward Gibson - 2008 - Cognition 108 (3):819-824.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   88 citations  
  • Neonatal Imitation: Theory, Experimental Design, and Significance for the Field of Social Cognition.Stefano Vincini, Yuna Jhang, Eugene H. Buder & Shaun Gallagher - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Robust representation of shape in a Grey parrot.Irene M. Pepperberg & Ken Nakayama - 2016 - Cognition 153 (C):146-160.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Subitizing and the FINST spatial index model.L. Trick & Z. Pylyshyn - 1989 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 27 (6):490-490.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The codes of man and beasts.David Premack - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):125-136.
    Exposing the chimpanzee to language training appears to enhance the animal's ability to perform some kinds of tasks but not others. The abilities that are enhanced involve abstract judgment, as in analogical reasoning, matching proportions of physically unlike exemplars, and completing incomplete representations of action. The abilities that do not improve concern the location of items in space and the inferences one might make in attempting to obtain them. Representing items in space and making inferences about them could be done (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   191 citations  
  • Effects of exposure time and intertrial interval upon decrement to the Müller-Lyer illusion.Paul T. Mountjoy - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 56 (2):97.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The comparative psychology of intelligence.Euan M. Macphail - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):645.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   193 citations  
  • Two experiments are reported in which the decline or decrement in the magnitude of the Brentano Müller-Lyer illusion was measured. Observers made a pre-test judgment and, after a variable intervening time period, a post-test judgment of illusion magnitude. In experiment 1, the intervening time periods were 1, 2, and 3 min during which time the independent groups of observers allocated to each of the three time periods either systematically scanned the Brentano figure (inspection conditions) or waited until the intervening period had elapsed (no-inspection conditions). Experiment 2, which included an additional 5 min intervening time period, evaluated a response-bias explanation for the results of the inspection conditions of experiment 1. Taken together, the findings of the two experiments indicate that sheer inspection of the Brentano figure produces illusion decrement. However, illusion decrement was independent of the duration of the inspection period, with equivalent amounts of dec. [REVIEW]J. Predebon - 1998 - Perception 27:183-192.
    Two experiments are reported in which the decline or decrement in the magnitude of the Brentano Müller-Lyer illusion was measured. Observers made a pre-test judgment and, after a variable intervening time period, a post-test judgment of illusion magnitude. In experiment 1, the intervening time periods were 1, 2, and 3 min during which time the independent groups of observers allocated to each of the three time periods either systematically scanned the Brentano figure (inspection conditions) or waited until the intervening period (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation