Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Measuring Individual Differences in Implicit Cognition: The Implicit Association Test.Debbie E. McGhee, Jordan L. K. Schwartz & Anthony G. Greenwald - 1998 - Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 (6):1464-1480.
    An implicit association test (IAT) measures differential association of 2 target concepts with an attribute. The 2 concepts appear in a 2-choice task (e.g., flower vs. insect names), and the attribute in a 2nd task (e.g., pleasant vs. unpleasant words for an evaluation attribute). When instructions oblige highly associated categories (e.g., flower + pleasant) to share a response key, performance is faster than when less associated categories (e.g., insect + pleasant) share a key. This performance difference implicitly measures differential association (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   285 citations  
  • The temporal structure of spoken language understanding.William Marslen-Wilson & Lorraine Komisarjevsky Tyler - 1980 - Cognition 8 (1):1-71.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   123 citations  
  • Representing word meaning and order information in a composite holographic lexicon.Michael N. Jones & Douglas J. K. Mewhort - 2007 - Psychological Review 114 (1):1-37.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   79 citations  
  • On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning.Ken McRae, Virginia R. de Sa & Mark S. Seidenberg - 1997 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 126 (2):99-130.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   67 citations  
  • Principles of categorization.Eleanor Rosch - 1988 - In Allan Collins & Edward E. Smith (eds.), Readings in Cognitive Science, a Perspective From Psychology and Artificial Intelligence. Morgan Kaufmann Publishers. pp. 312-22.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   364 citations  
  • A Taxonomy of Part‐Whole Relations.Morton E. Winston, Roger Chaffin & Douglas Herrmann - 1987 - Cognitive Science 11 (4):417-444.
    A taxonomy of part‐whole or meronymic relations is developed to explain the ordinary English‐speaker's use of the term “part of” and its cognates. The resulting classification yields six types of meronymic relations: 1. component‐integral object (pedal‐bike), 2. member‐collection (ship‐fleet), 3. portion‐mass (slice‐pie), 4. stuff‐object (steel‐car), 5. feature‐activity (paying‐shopping), and 6. place‐area (Everglades‐Florida). Meronymic relations ore further distinguished from other inclusion relations, such as spatial inclusion, and class inclusion, and from several other semantic relations: attribution, attachment, and ownership. This taxonomy is (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • Word meaning and the control of eye fixation: semantic competitor effects and the visual world paradigm.Falk Huettig & Gerry T. M. Altmann - 2005 - Cognition 96 (1):B23-B32.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   50 citations  
  • Diagnostic Experimental Philosophy.Eugen Fischer & Paul E. Engelhardt - 2017 - Teorema: International Journal of Philosophy 36 (3):117-137.
    Experimental philosophy’s much-discussed ‘restrictionist’ program seeks to delineate the extent to which philosophers may legitimately rely on intuitions about possible cases. The present paper shows that this program can be (i) put to the service of diagnostic problem-resolution (in the wake of J.L. Austin) and (ii) pursued by constructing and experimentally testing psycholinguistic explanations of intuitions which expose their lack of evidentiary value: The paper develops a psycholinguistic explanation of paradoxical intuitions that are prompted by verbal case-descriptions, and presents two (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Visual and Affective Multimodal Models of Word Meaning in Language and Mind.Simon De Deyne, Danielle J. Navarro, Guillem Collell & Andrew Perfors - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (1):e12922.
    One of the main limitations of natural language‐based approaches to meaning is that they do not incorporate multimodal representations the way humans do. In this study, we evaluate how well different kinds of models account for people's representations of both concrete and abstract concepts. The models we compare include unimodal distributional linguistic models as well as multimodal models which combine linguistic with perceptual or affective information. There are two types of linguistic models: those based on text corpora and those derived (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • What some concepts might not be.Sharon Lee Armstrong, Lila R. Gleitman & Henry Gleitman - 1983 - Cognition 13 (1):263--308.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   242 citations  
  • N400s from sentences, semantic categories, number and letter strings?John Polich - 1985 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 23 (4):361-364.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Skills of divided attention.Elizabeth Spelke - 1976 - Cognition 4 (3):215-230.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Linguistic Distributional Knowledge and Sensorimotor Grounding both Contribute to Semantic Category Production.Briony Banks, Cai Wingfield & Louise Connell - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (10):e13055.
    The human conceptual system comprises simulated information of sensorimotor experience and linguistic distributional information of how words are used in language. Moreover, the linguistic shortcut hypothesis predicts that people will use computationally cheaper linguistic distributional information where it is sufficient to inform a task response. In a pre‐registered category production study, we asked participants to verbally name members of concrete and abstract categories and tested whether performance could be predicted by a novel measure of sensorimotor similarity (based on an 11‐dimensional (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Evolution and devolution of folkbiological knowledge.Phillip Wolff, Douglas L. Medin & Connie Pankratz - 1999 - Cognition 73 (2):177-204.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • On the equivalence of superordinate concepts.Edward J. Wisniewski, Mutsumi Imai & Lyman Casey - 1996 - Cognition 60 (3):269-298.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Self-Reported Stickiness of Mind-Wandering Affects Task Performance.Marieke K. van Vugt & Nico Broers - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Relationships among goodness-of-example, category norms, and word frequency.Carolyn B. Mervis, Jack Catlin & Eleanor Rosch - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 7 (3):283-284.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Convergent behavioral and neuropsychological evidence for a distinction between identification and production forms of repetition priming.John De Gabrieli, Chandan J. Vaidya, Maria Stone, Wendy S. Francis, Sharon L. Thompson-Schill, Debra A. Fleischman, Jared R. Tinklenberg, Jerome A. Yesavage & Robert S. Wilson - 1999 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128 (4):479.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Memory bias for negative emotional words in recognition memory is driven by effects of category membership.Corey N. White, Aycan Kapucu, Davide Bruno, Caren M. Rotello & Roger Ratcliff - 2014 - Cognition and Emotion 28 (5):867-880.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The Cognitive Structure of Social Categories.Kathleen Dahlgren - 1985 - Cognitive Science 9 (3):379-398.
    Support for the prototype theory of categorization was found in a study of the structure of social categories. Though occupational terms such as DOCTOR are socially defined, they do not have the classical structure their clear definitional origins would predict. Conceptions of social categories are richer and more complex than those of physical object categories and subjects agree upon them. Comparison of various instructions for eliciting attributes of categories showed that whether subjects are asked to define a term, give characteristics, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Implicit memory bias, explicit memory bias, and anxiety.Michael W. Eysenck & Angela Byrne - 1994 - Cognition and Emotion 8 (5):415-431.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Speech intelligibility and recall of first and second language words heard at different signal-to-noise ratios.Staffan Hygge, Anders Kjellberg & Anatole Nöstl - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Metacognitive awareness of event-based prospective memory☆.J. Thadeus Meeks, Jason L. Hicks & Richard L. Marsh - 2007 - Consciousness and Cognition 16 (4):997-1004.
    This study examined people’s ability to predict and postdict their performance on an event-based prospective memory task. Using nonfocal cues, one group of participants predicted their success at finding animal words and a different group predicted their ability to find words with a particular syllable in it. The authors also administered a self-report questionnaire on everyday prospective and retrospective memory failures. Based on the different strategies adopted by the two groups and correlations among the dependent variables, the authors concluded that (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Motor system contributions to verbal and non-verbal working memory.Diana A. Liao, Sharif I. Kronemer, Jeffrey M. Yau, John E. Desmond & Cherie L. Marvel - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Reliability of retrieval from semantic memory: Common categories.Francis S. Bellezza - 1984 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 22 (4):324-326.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Anxiety and retrieval inhibition: support for an enhanced inhibition account.Mia Nuñez, Josh Gregory & Richard E. Zinbarg - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (2).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Is the Implicit Association Test a Valid and Valuable Measure of Implicit Consumer Social Cognition?Brian C. Tietje - unknown
    This article discusses the need for more satisfactory implicit measures in consumer psychology and assesses the theoretical foundations, validity, and value of the Implicit Association Test (IAT) as a measure of implicit consumer social cognition. Study 1 demonstrates the IAT’s sensitivity to explicit individual differences in brand attitudes, ownership, and usage frequency, and shows their correlations with IAT-based measures of implicit brand attitudes and brand relationship strength. In Study 2, the contrast between explicit and implicit measures of attitude toward the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • When social influences reduce false recognition memory: A case of categorically related information.Suparna Rajaram, Raeya Maswood & Luciane P. Pereira-Pasarin - 2020 - Cognition 202:104279.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Cross‐Cultural Differences in Categorical Memory Errors.Aliza J. Schwartz, Aysecan Boduroglu & Angela H. Gutchess - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (5):997-1007.
    Cultural differences occur in the use of categories to aid accurate recall of information. This study investigated whether culture also contributed to false (erroneous) memories, and extended cross-cultural memory research to Turkish culture, which is shaped by Eastern and Western influences. Americans and Turks viewed word pairs, half of which were categorically related and half unrelated. Participants then attempted to recall the second word from the pair in response to the first word cue. Responses were coded as correct, as blanks, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Using Wikipedia to learn semantic feature representations of concrete concepts in neuroimaging experiments.Francisco Pereira, Matthew Botvinick & Greg Detre - 2013 - Artificial Intelligence 194 (C):240-252.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Reliability of retrieval from semantic memory: Noun meanings.Francis S. Bellezza - 1984 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 22 (5):377-380.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Verifying autobiographical facts.M. A. Conway - 1987 - Cognition 26 (1):39-58.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Part-set cuing inhibition in category-instance and reason generation.Steven A. Sloman - 1991 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 29 (2):136-138.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Acquiring New Factual Information: Effect of Prior Knowledge.Haoyu Chen, Xueling Ning, Lingwei Wang & Jiongjiong Yang - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Conceptual implicit memory and environmental context.Neil W. Mulligan - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (3):737-744.
    Changes in environmental context between encoding and retrieval often affect explicit memory but research on implicit memory is equivocal. One proposal is that conceptual but not perceptual priming is influenced by context manipulations. However, findings with conceptual priming may be compromised by explicit contamination. The present study examined the effects of environmental context on conceptual explicit and implicit memory . Explicit recall was reduced by context change. The implicit test results depended on test awareness . Among test-unaware participants, priming was (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Locating what comes to mind in empirically derived representational spaces.Tracey Mills & Jonathan Phillips - 2023 - Cognition 240 (C):105549.
    Real-world judgements and decisions often require choosing from an open-ended set of options which cannot be exhaustively considered before a choice is made. Recent work has found that the options people do consider tend to have particular features, such as high historical value. Here, we pursue the idea that option generation during decision making may reflect a more general mechanism for calling things to mind, by which relevant features in a context-appropriate representational space guide what comes to mind. In this (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Preschool period development of implicit and explicit remembering.Julia L. Greenbaum & Peter Graf - 1989 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 27 (5):417-420.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • “Robins are a part of birds”: The confusion of semantic relations.Douglas J. Herrmann, Roger Chaffin & Morton E. Winston - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (6):413-415.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Lost thoughts: Implicit semantic interference impairs reflective access to currently active information.Julie A. Higgins & Marcia K. Johnson - 2013 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142 (1):6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Examining the costs and benefits of inhibition in memory retrieval.Christopher J. Schilling, Benjamin C. Storm & Michael C. Anderson - 2014 - Cognition 133 (2):358-370.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • When does a good working memory counteract proactive interference? Surprising evidence from a probe recognition task.Nelson Cowan & J. Scott Saults - 2013 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142 (1):12.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Inference Is Bliss: Using Evolutionary Relationship to Guide Categorical Inferences.Laura R. Novick, Kefyn M. Catley & Daniel J. Funk - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (4):712-743.
    Three experiments, adopting an evolutionary biology perspective, investigated subjects’ inferences about living things. Subjects were told that different enzymes help regulate cell function in two taxa and asked which enzyme a third taxon most likely uses. Experiment 1 and its follow-up, with college students, used triads involving amphibians, reptiles, and mammals (reptiles and mammals are most closely related evolutionarily) and plants, fungi, and animals (fungi are more closely related to animals than to plants). Experiment 2, with 10th graders, also included (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Age differences in free recall and clustering as a function of list length and trials.Susan Brown-Whistler & Joel S. Freund - 1993 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 31 (1):7-10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Context facilitation and disruption in word identification.Scott W. Brown & Richard A. Block - 1980 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 15 (4):242-244.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Memory under anesthesia: Evidence for response suppression.Alan S. Brown, Michael R. Best, David B. Mitchell & Lloyd C. Haggard - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (3):244-246.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Part-list reexposure and release of retrieval inhibition.B. H. Basden, D. R. Basden & M. J. Wright - 2003 - Consciousness and Cognition 12 (3):354-375.
    In list-method directed forgetting, reexposure to forgotten List 1 items has been shown to reduce directed forgetting. proposed that reexposure to a few List 1 items only during a direct test of memory reinstates the entire List 1 episode. In the present experiments, part-list reexposure in the context of indirect as well as direct memory tests reduced directed forgetting. Directed forgetting was reduced when 50% or more of the items were reexposed, and was intact when only 25% were reexposed. Furthermore, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • When lack of practice helps: Retrieval following single presentations of categorized lists.David Burrows - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (6):404-406.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Implicit contributions of context to recognition.D. Manier - 2004 - Consciousness and Cognition 13 (3):471-483.
    Studies of the impact of context on remembering have not focused on the influence of contextual contingency on subsequent recognition in the condition in which the contingency cannot be verbalized. In two experiments, we analyzed the effect of an implicitly encoded position contingency involving location and semantic category on both hit and false alarm recognition judgments after 1 day and 1 week delays. We vigorously probed for what participants could say about the contingency. We found context effects for both hits (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Effects of frequency on retrieval from a semantic category.Carl P. Duncan - 1977 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 10 (1):57-59.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Prodded retrieval from a semantic category.Carl P. Duncan - 1975 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 6 (1):61-62.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation