Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Making tools isn’t child’s play.Sarah R. Beck, Ian A. Apperly, Jackie Chappell, Carlie Guthrie & Nicola Cutting - 2011 - Cognition 119 (2):301-306.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • The role of functional information for infant categorization.Birgit Träuble & Sabina Pauen - 2007 - Cognition 105 (2):362-379.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The Role of Book Features in Young Children's Transfer of Information from Picture Books to Real-World Contexts.Gabrielle A. Strouse, Angela Nyhout & Patricia A. Ganea - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:299131.
    Picture books are an important source of new language, concepts, and lessons for young children. A large body of research has documented the nature of parent-child interactions during shared book reading. A new body of research has begun to investigate the features of picture books that support children's learning and transfer of that information to the real world. In this paper, we discuss how children's symbolic development, analogical reasoning, and reasoning about fantasy may constrain their ability to take away content (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Recognition and categorization of biologically significant objects by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): the domain of food.Elizabeth Spelke - 2001 - Cognition 82 (2):127-155.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism?Linda B. Smith, Susan S. Jones & Barbara Landau - 1996 - Cognition 60 (2):143-171.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Core knowledge and its limits: The domain of food.Kristin Shutts, Kirsten F. Condry, Laurie R. Santos & Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2009 - Cognition 112 (1):120-140.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Children's judgments in theory choice tasks: Scientific rationality in childhood.Ala Samarapungavan - 1992 - Cognition 45 (1):1-32.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The moral, or the story? Changing children's distributive justice preferences through social communication.Joshua Rottman, Valerie Zizik, Kelly Minard, Liane Young, Peter R. Blake & Deborah Kelemen - 2020 - Cognition 205 (C):104441.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Comparison of active and purely visual performance in a multiple-string means-end task in infants.Lauriane Rat-Fischer, J. Kevin O’Regan & Jacqueline Fagard - 2014 - Cognition 133 (1):304-316.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Young children’s tool innovation across culture: Affordance visibility matters.Karri Neldner, Ilana Mushin & Mark Nielsen - 2017 - Cognition 168:335-343.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • What Human Planning Can Tell Us About Animal Planning: An Empirical Case.Gema Martin-Ordas - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the Spatial Foundations of the Conceptual System and Its Enrichment.Jean M. Mandler - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (3):421-451.
    A theory of how concept formation begins is presented that accounts for conceptual activity in the first year of life, shows how increasing conceptual complexity comes about, and predicts the order in which new types of information accrue to the conceptual system. In a compromise between nativist and empiricist views, it offers a single domain-general mechanism that redescribes attended spatiotemporal information into an iconic form. The outputs of this mechanism consist of types of spatial information that we know infants attend (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Two dogmas of conceptual empiricism: implications for hybrid models of the structure of knowledge.Frank Keil - 1998 - Cognition 65 (2-3):103-135.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   46 citations  
  • Do children have a theory of race?Lawrence A. Hirschfeld - 1995 - Cognition 54 (2):209-252.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Artifactual kinds and functional design features: what a primate understands without language.Marc D. Hauser - 1997 - Cognition 64 (3):285-308.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Sex differences in young children’s use of tools in a problem-solving task.Jeffrey M. Gredlein & David F. Bjorklund - 2005 - Human Nature 16 (2):211-232.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Meaningful questions: The acquisition of auxiliary inversion in a connectionist model of sentence production.Hartmut Fitz & Franklin Chang - 2017 - Cognition 166 (C):225-250.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • No interpretation without representation: the role of domain-specific representations and inferences in the Wason selection task.Laurence Fiddick, Leda Cosmides & John Tooby - 2000 - Cognition 77 (1):1-79.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   62 citations  
  • What Does It Take for an Infant to Learn How to Use a Tool by Observation?Jacqueline Fagard, Lauriane Rat-Fischer, Rana Esseily, Eszter Somogyi & J. K. O’Regan - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The emergence of use of a rake-like tool: a longitudinal study in human infants.Jacqueline Fagard, Lauriane Rat-Fischer & J. Kevin O'Regan - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • In defense of epicycles: Embracing complexity in psychological explanations.Ansgar D. Endress - 2023 - Mind and Language 38 (5):1208-1237.
    Is formal simplicity a guide to learning in humans, as simplicity is said to be a guide to the acceptability of theories in science? Does simplicity determine the difficulty of various learning tasks? I argue that, similarly to how scientists sometimes preferred complex theories when this facilitated calculations, results from perception, learning and reasoning suggest that formal complexity is generally unrelated to what is easy to learn and process by humans, and depends on assumptions about available representational and processing primitives. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Artifact and Tool Categorization.Sara Dellantonio, Claudio Mulatti & Remo Job - 2013 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 4 (3):407-418.
    This study addresses the issue of artifact kinds from a psychological and cognitive perspective. The primary interest of the investigation lies in understanding how artifacts are categorized and what are the properties people rely on for their identification. According to a classical philosophical definition artifacts form an autonomous class of instances including all and only those objects that do not exist in nature, but are artificial, in the sense that they are made by an artĭfex. This definition suggests that artifacts (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Acquiring an understanding of design: evidence from children's insight problem solving.Margaret Anne Defeyter & Tim P. German - 2003 - Cognition 89 (2):133-155.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • A developmental dissociation between category and function judgments about novel artifacts.Margaret A. Defeyter, Jill Hearing & Tamsin C. German - 2009 - Cognition 110 (2):260-264.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Beyond intuition and instinct blindness: Toward an evolutionary rigorous cognitive science.Leda Cosmides & John Tooby - 1994 - Cognition 50 (1-3):41-77.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   93 citations  
  • Where science starts: Spontaneous experiments in preschoolers’ exploratory play.Claire Cook, Noah D. Goodman & Laura E. Schulz - 2011 - Cognition 120 (3):341-349.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • The cognizer's innards: A psychological and philosophical perspective on the development of thought.Andy Clark & Annette Karmiloff-Smith - 1993 - Mind and Language 8 (4):487-519.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   164 citations  
  • Function is not the sum of an object’s parts.Krista Casler - 2018 - Thinking and Reasoning 25 (3):300-323.
    Prior research shows adults believe objects exist for specialised purposes. This “one tool, one function” cognitive bias promotes efficient mastery of artefact function but could mean indiv...
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Doing Without Schema Hierarchies: A Recurrent Connectionist Approach to Normal and Impaired Routine Sequential Action.Matthew Botvinick & David C. Plaut - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (2):395-429.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   54 citations  
  • The cause of infant categorization?Amy E. Booth - 2008 - Cognition 106 (2):984-993.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • How biological is essentialism.Susan A. Gelman & Lawrence A. Hirschfeld - 1999 - In Douglas L. Medin & Scott Atran (eds.), Folkbiology. MIT Press. pp. 403--446.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • Conceptual Analysis Naturalized.Christopher Hitchcock - 2006 - Journal of Philosophy 103 (9):427-451.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Learning science through inquiry in kindergarten.Ala Samarapungavan, Panayota Mantzicopoulos & Helen Patrick - 2008 - Science Education 92 (5):868-908.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark