Switch to: References

Citations of:

What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of language in his first language

In T. E. Moore (ed.), Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language. Academic. pp. 65--110 (1973)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Word learning does not end at fast-mapping: Evolution of verb meanings through reorganization of an entire semantic domain.Noburo Saji, Mutsumi Imai, Henrik Saalbach, Yuping Zhang, Hua Shu & Hiroyuki Okada - 2011 - Cognition 118 (1):45-61.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Semantic components and conceptual development.Jonathan Baron - 1973 - Cognition 2 (3):299-317.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • From communication to language—a psychological perspective.J. S. Bruner - 1974 - Cognition 3 (3):255-287.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   64 citations  
  • Reference in memorial tribute to Eric Lenneberg.Roger Brown - 1976 - Cognition 4 (2):125-153.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Whence and whither in spatial language and spatial cognition?Barbara Landau & Ray Jackendoff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):255-265.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Is spatial information imprecise or just coarsely coded?P. Bryan Heidorn & Stephen C. Hirtle - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):246-247.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Evolution and physiology of “what” versus “where”.David Ingle - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):247-248.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition.Barbara Landau & Ray Jackendoff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):217-238.
    Fundamental to spatial knowledge in all species are the representations underlying object recognition, object search, and navigation through space. But what sets humans apart from other species is our ability to express spatial experience through language. This target article explores the language ofobjectsandplaces, asking what geometric properties are preserved in the representations underlying object nouns and spatial prepositions in English. Evidence from these two aspects of language suggests there are significant differences in the geometric richness with which objects and places (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   101 citations  
  • The Objective Confirmation of Hypotheses.Nathan Stemmer - 1981 - Canadian Journal of Philosophy 11 (3):395 - 404.
    According to Nicod's criterion of confirmation, a hypothesis of the form ‘All P are Q’ is confirmed by its positive instances, i.e. by entities that are P and Q. It is well known, however, that the criterion gives origin to counterintuitive results. For example, it sanctions the confirmation of ‘All nonblacks are nonravens’ by a nonblack nonraven. Since this hypothesis is logically equivalent to ‘All ravens are black,’ it follows that the criterion indirectly sanctions the counterintuitive confirmation of ‘All ravens (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Concepts and Cognitive Science.Stephen Laurence & Eric Margolis - 1999 - In Eric Margolis & Stephen Laurence (eds.), Concepts: Core Readings. MIT Press. pp. 3-81.
    Given the fundamental role that concepts play in theories of cognition, philosophers and cognitive scientists have a common interest in concepts. Nonetheless, there is a great deal of controversy regarding what kinds of things concepts are, how they are structured, and how they are acquired. This chapter offers a detailed high-level overview and critical evaluation of the main theories of concepts and their motivations. Taking into account the various challenges that each theory faces, the chapter also presents a novel approach (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   253 citations  
  • The link between brain learning, attention, and consciousness.Stephen Grossberg - 1999 - Consciousness and Cognition 8 (1):1-44.
    The processes whereby our brains continue to learn about a changing world in a stable fashion throughout life are proposed to lead to conscious experiences. These processes include the learning of top-down expectations, the matching of these expectations against bottom-up data, the focusing of attention upon the expected clusters of information, and the development of resonant states between bottom-up and top-down processes as they reach an attentive consensus between what is expected and what is there in the outside world. It (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   57 citations  
  • The Characterization of Concepts in a Metalanguage for Lexicographic Semantics.Keith Allan - forthcoming - Topoi:1-14.
    A successful lexicographic semantic description characterizes the concepts that occur in establishing the meaning of a listeme by modelling what the competent native-like speaker of the language knows. Because concepts can only be identified to another human through the medium of a natural language, the metalanguage used in the semantic definition of a natural language expression in the object language will always be equivalent to the natural language expression through which that metalanguage is interpreted. The metalanguage of a semantic theory (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Signing behavior in apes: A critical review.Mark S. Seidenberg & Laura A. Petitto - 1979 - Cognition 7 (2):177-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   146 citations  
  • Non-linguistic strategies and the acquisition of word meanings.Eve V. Clark - 1973 - Cognition 2 (2):161-182.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  • Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism?Linda B. Smith, Susan S. Jones & Barbara Landau - 1996 - Cognition 60 (2):143-171.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Shareability: The Social Psychology of Epistemology.Jennifer J. Freyd - 1983 - Cognitive Science 7 (3):191-210.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   73 citations  
  • On the Spatial Foundations of the Conceptual System and Its Enrichment.Jean M. Mandler - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (3):421-451.
    A theory of how concept formation begins is presented that accounts for conceptual activity in the first year of life, shows how increasing conceptual complexity comes about, and predicts the order in which new types of information accrue to the conceptual system. In a compromise between nativist and empiricist views, it offers a single domain-general mechanism that redescribes attended spatiotemporal information into an iconic form. The outputs of this mechanism consist of types of spatial information that we know infants attend (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Concepts and nativism.Nicholas Adamson - unknown
    Jerry Fodor has argued that virtually all lexical concepts are innate. I argue against this position, but not, as other have done, on the grounds that the arguments against lexical decomposition upon which Fodor relies are flawed. Rather, I argue that even if lexical concepts cannot be decomposed, the possession conditions for having lexical concepts are nonetheless not innately satisfied.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Holism, language acquisition, and algebraic logic.Eli Dresner - 2002 - Linguistics and Philosophy 25 (4):419-452.
    In the first section of this paper I present a well known objection to meaning holism, according to which holism is inconsistent with natural language being learnable. Then I show that the objection fails if language acquisition includes stages of partial grasp of the meaning of at least some expressions, and I argue that standard model theoretic semantics cannot fully capture such stages. In the second section the above claims are supported through a review of current research into language acquisition. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Basic-level categorization and part-whole perception in children.Ruth Kimchi - 1993 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 31 (1):23-26.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “In”, “on”, and “under” revisited.Stephen Wilcox & David S. Palermo - 1974 - Cognition 3 (3):245-254.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • The colour cognition of children.Jules Davidoff & Peter Mitchell - 1993 - Cognition 48 (2):121-137.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • A reassessment of the shift from the classical theory of concepts to prototype theory.Eric Margolis - 1994 - Cognition 51 (1):73-89.
    A standard view within psychology is that there have been two important shifts in the study of concepts and that each has led to some improvements. The first shift was from the classical theory of concepts to probabilistic theories, including the prototype theory. The second shift was from probabilistic theories to theory-based theories. In this article, I critically evaluate the view that the first shift was a major advance and argue that the prototype theory suffers some of the same problems (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • From perception to cognition.Michael J. Tarr - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):251-252.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the Experiential Link Between Spatial and Temporal Language.Teenie Matlock, Michael Ramscar & Lera Boroditsky - 2005 - Cognitive Science 29 (4):655-664.
    How do we understand time and other entities we can neither touch nor see? One possibility is that we tap into our concrete, experiential knowledge, including our understanding of physical space and motion, to make sense of abstract domains such as time. To examine how pervasive an aspect of cognition this is, we investigated whether thought about a nonliteral type of motion called fictive motion (FM; as in The road runs along the coast) can influence thought about time. Our results (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Do Metaphors Move From Mind to Mouth? Evidence From a New System of Linguistic Metaphors for Time.Rose K. Hendricks, Benjamin K. Bergen & Tyler Marghetis - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (8):2950-2975.
    Languages around the world use a recurring strategy to discuss abstract concepts: describe them metaphorically, borrowing language from more concrete domains. We “plan ahead” to the future, “count up” to higher numbers, and “warm” to new friends. Past work has found that these ways of talking have implications for how we think, so that shared systems of linguistic metaphors can produce shared conceptualizations. On the other hand, these systematic linguistic metaphors might not just be the cause but also the effect (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Language in the two-year old.Susan Goldin-Meadow, Martin E. P. Seligman & Rochel Gelman - 1976 - Cognition 4 (2):189-202.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: Object terms and substance terms.Nancy N. Soja, Susan Carey & Elizabeth S. Spelke - 1991 - Cognition 38 (2):179-211.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   82 citations  
  • The role of cerebral lateralization in expression of spatial cognition.Halle D. Brown - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):240-241.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Induction of Augmented Transition Networks.John R. Anderson - 1977 - Cognitive Science 1 (2):125-157.
    LAS is a program that acquires augmented transition network (ATN) grammars. It requires as data sentences of the language and semantic network representatives of their meaning. In acquiring the ATN grammars, it induces the word classes of the language, the rules of formation for sentences, and the rules mapping sentences onto meaning. The induced ATN grammar can be used both for sentence generation and sentence comprehension. Critical to the performance of the program are assumptions that it makes about the relation (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Representing Space in Language and Perception.David J. Bryant - 1997 - Mind and Language 12 (3-4):239-264.
    Space can be understood through perception and language, but are the processes that represent spatial information the same in both cases? This paper reviews psychological evidence for the functional equivalence of spatial representations based on perceptual and linguistic inputs. It is proposed that spatial information is processed by a specialised spatial representation system (SRS) that creates geometric representations of space. The SRS receives inputs from perceptual and linguistic systems and uses these basic inputs to construct mental spatial models of the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • A Modular Neural Network Model of Concept Acquisition.Philippe G. Schyns - 1991 - Cognitive Science 15 (4):461-508.
    Previous neural network models of concept learning were mainly implemented with supervised learning schemes. However, studies of human conceptual memory have shown that concepts may be learned without a teacher who provides the category name to associate with exemplars. A modular neural network architecture that realizes concept acquisition through two functionally distinct operations, categorizing and naming, is proposed as an alternative. An unsupervised algorithm realizes the categorizing module by constructing representations of categories compatible with prototype theory. The naming module associates (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • (1 other version)Empiricist versus prototype theories of language acquisition.Nathan Stemmer - 1989 - Mind and Language 4 (3):201-221.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Concepts and words in the 18-month-old: Acquiring concept names under controlled conditions.Keith E. Nelson & John D. Bonvillian - 1973 - Cognition 2 (4):435-450.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Some attributes of adjectives used by young children.Katherine Nelson - 1976 - Cognition 4 (1):13-30.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Spatial and cognitive vision differentiate at low levels, but not in language.Bruce Bridgeman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):240-240.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are spatial representations flattish?J. B. Deregowski - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):243-244.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Précis of how children learn the meanings of words.Paul Bloom - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (6):1095-1103.
    Normal children learn tens of thousands of words, and do so quickly and efficiently, often in highly impoverished environments. In How Children Learn the Meanings of Words, I argue that word learning is the product of certain cognitive and linguistic abilities that include the ability to acquire concepts, an appreciation of syntactic cues to meaning, and a rich understanding of the mental states of other people. These capacities are powerful, early emerging, and to some extent uniquely human, but they are (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Generative versus nongenerative thought.Michael C. Corballis - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):242-243.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The development of features in object concepts.Philippe G. Schyns, Robert L. Goldstone & Jean-Pierre Thibaut - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (1):1-17.
    According to one productive and influential approach to cognition, categorization, object recognition, and higher level cognitive processes operate on a set of fixed features, which are the output of lower level perceptual processes. In many situations, however, it is the higher level cognitive process being executed that influences the lower level features that are created. Rather than viewing the repertoire of features as being fixed by low-level processes, we present a theory in which people create features to subserve the representation (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   99 citations  
  • Word associations and the development of lexical memory.Sandy Petrey - 1977 - Cognition 5 (1):57-71.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Labels as Features (Not Names) for Infant Categorization: A Neurocomputational Approach.Valentina Gliozzi, Julien Mayor, Jon-Fan Hu & Kim Plunkett - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (4):709-738.
    A substantial body of experimental evidence has demonstrated that labels have an impact on infant categorization processes. Yet little is known regarding the nature of the mechanisms by which this effect is achieved. We distinguish between two competing accounts: supervised name‐based categorization and unsupervised feature‐based categorization. We describe a neurocomputational model of infant visual categorization, based on self‐organizing maps, that implements the unsupervised feature‐based approach. The model successfully reproduces experiments demonstrating the impact of labeling on infant visual categorization reported in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • What Does Children's Spatial Language Reveal About Spatial Concepts? Evidence From the Use of Containment Expressions.Megan Johanson & Anna Papafragou - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (5):881-910.
    Children's overextensions of spatial language are often taken to reveal spatial biases. However, it is unclear whether extension patterns should be attributed to children's overly general spatial concepts or to a narrower notion of conceptual similarity allowing metaphor‐like extensions. We describe a previously unnoticed extension of spatial expressions and use a novel method to determine its origins. English‐ and Greek‐speaking 4‐ and 5‐year‐olds used containment expressions (e.g., Englishinto, Greekmesa) for events where an object moved into another object but extended such (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Simplicity and Generalization: Short-cutting Abstraction in Children's Object Categorizations.Robert L. Goldstone Ji Y. Son, Linda B. Smith - 2008 - Cognition 108 (3):626.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Grammatical Morphemes and Conceptual Structure in Discourse Processing.Daniel G. Morrow - 1986 - Cognitive Science 10 (4):423-455.
    The present paper analyzes how the semantic and pragmatic functions of closed class categories, or grammatical morphemes (i.e., inflections and function words), organize discourse processing. Grammatical morphemes tend to express a small set of conceptual distinctions that organize a wide range of objects and relations, usually expressed by content or open class words (i.e., nouns and verbs), into situations anchored to a discourse context. Therefore, grammatical morphemes and content words cooperate in guiding the construction of a situation model during discourse (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Uses of cognitive science to science education.W. Jung - 1993 - Science & Education 2 (1):31-56.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Talking to yourself about what is where: What is the vocabulary of preattentive vision?Jeremy M. Wolfe - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):254-255.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Prepositions aren't places.Barbara Tversky & Herbert H. Clark - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):252-253.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Ouroboros Model Embraces Its Sensory-Motoric Foundations And Learns To Talk.Knud Thomsen - 2015 - Studies in Logic, Grammar and Rhetoric 41 (1):105-125.
    The Ouroboros Model proposes a brain inspired cognitive architecture including detailed suggestions for the main processing steps in an overall conceptualization of cognition as embodied and embedded computing. All memories are structured into schemata, which are firmly grounded in the body of an actor. A cyclic and iterative data-acquisition and -processing loop forms the backbone of all cognitive activity. Ever more sophisticated schemata are built up incrementally from the wide combination of neural activity, concurrent at the point in time when (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • No perception without representation.Donald D. Hoffman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):247-247.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark