Switch to: References

Citations of:

Finding Structure in Time

Cognitive Science 14 (2):179-211 (1990)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. A Fodorian guide to Switzerland: Jung and Piaget combined?Péter Bodor & Csaba Pléh - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):709-710.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Implicit Learning and Acquisition of Music.Martin Rohrmeier & Patrick Rebuschat - 2012 - Topics in Cognitive Science 4 (4):525-553.
    Implicit learning is a core process for the acquisition of a complex, rule‐based environment from mere interaction, such as motor action, skill acquisition, or language. A body of evidence suggests that implicit knowledge governs music acquisition and perception in nonmusicians and musicians, and that both expert and nonexpert participants acquire complex melodic, harmonic, and other features from mere exposure. While current findings and computational modeling largely support the learning of chunks, some results indicate learning of more complex structures. Despite the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Learning a Generative Probabilistic Grammar of Experience: A Process‐Level Model of Language Acquisition.Oren Kolodny, Arnon Lotem & Shimon Edelman - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (4):227-267.
    We introduce a set of biologically and computationally motivated design choices for modeling the learning of language, or of other types of sequential, hierarchically structured experience and behavior, and describe an implemented system that conforms to these choices and is capable of unsupervised learning from raw natural-language corpora. Given a stream of linguistic input, our model incrementally learns a grammar that captures its statistical patterns, which can then be used to parse or generate new data. The grammar constructed in this (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Hierarchical models of behavior and prefrontal function.Matthew M. Botvinick - 2008 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12 (5):201.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   44 citations  
  • Perceptual constraints and the learnability of simple grammars.Ansgar D. Endress, Ghislaine Dehaene-Lambertz & Jacques Mehler - 2007 - Cognition 105 (3):577-614.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Connectionist Natural Language Processing: The State of the Art.Morten H. Christiansen & Nick Chater - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):417-437.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Distributional Information: A Powerful Cue for Acquiring Syntactic Categories.Martin Redington, Nick Chater & Steven Finch - 1998 - Cognitive Science 22 (4):425-469.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   64 citations  
  • Varieties of consciousness.Paolo Bartolomeo & Gianfranco Dalla Barba - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (3):331-332.
    In agreement with some of the ideas expressed by Perruchet & Vinter (P&V), we believe that some phenomena hitherto attributed to processing may in fact reflect a fundamental distinction between direct and reflexive forms of consciousness. This dichotomy, developed by the phenomenological tradition, is substantiated by examples coming from experimental psychology and lesion neuropsychology.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Learning biases predict a word order universal.Jennifer Culbertson, Paul Smolensky & Géraldine Legendre - 2012 - Cognition 122 (3):306-329.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   46 citations  
  • Mechanisms for the generation and regulation of sequential behaviour.Richard P. Cooper - 2003 - Philosophical Psychology 16 (3):389 – 416.
    A critical aspect of much human behaviour is the generation and regulation of sequential activities. Such behaviour is seen in both naturalistic settings such as routine action and language production and laboratory tasks such as serial recall and many reaction time experiments. There are a variety of computational mechanisms that may support the generation and regulation of sequential behaviours, ranging from those underlying Turing machines to those employed by recurrent connectionist networks. This paper surveys a range of such mechanisms, together (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Currents in connectionism.William Bechtel - 1993 - Minds and Machines 3 (2):125-153.
    This paper reviews four significant advances on the feedforward architecture that has dominated discussions of connectionism. The first involves introducing modularity into networks by employing procedures whereby different networks learn to perform different components of a task, and a Gating Network determines which network is best equiped to respond to a given input. The second consists in the use of recurrent inputs whereby information from a previous cycle of processing is made available on later cycles. The third development involves developing (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Cognizers' innards and connectionist nets: A holy alliance?Adele Abrahamsen - 1993 - Mind and Language 8 (4):520-530.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The case for connectionism.William Bechtel - 1993 - Philosophical Studies 71 (2):119-54.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The path beyond first-order connectionism.William Bechtel - 1993 - Mind and Language 8 (4):531-539.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Predicting Age of Acquisition for Children's Early Vocabulary in Five Languages Using Language Model Surprisal.Eva Portelance, Yuguang Duan, Michael C. Frank & Gary Lupyan - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (9):e13334.
    What makes a word easy to learn? Early‐learned words are frequent and tend to name concrete referents. But words typically do not occur in isolation. Some words are predictable from their contexts; others are less so. Here, we investigate whether predictability relates to when children start producing different words (age of acquisition; AoA). We operationalized predictability in terms of a word's surprisal in child‐directed speech, computed using n‐gram and long‐short‐term‐memory (LSTM) language models. Predictability derived from LSTMs was generally a better (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Word Senses as Clusters of Meaning Modulations: A Computational Model of Polysemy.Jiangtian Li & Marc F. Joanisse - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (4):e12955.
    Most words in natural languages are polysemous; that is, they have related but different meanings in different contexts. This one‐to‐many mapping of form to meaning presents a challenge to understanding how word meanings are learned, represented, and processed. Previous work has focused on solutions in which multiple static semantic representations are linked to a single word form, which fails to capture important generalizations about how polysemous words are used; in particular, the graded nature of polysemous senses, and the flexibility and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Reinforcement learning of non-Markov decision processes.Steven D. Whitehead & Long-Ji Lin - 1995 - Artificial Intelligence 73 (1-2):271-306.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Prediction‐Based Learning and Processing of Event Knowledge.Ken McRae, Kevin S. Brown & Jeffrey L. Elman - 2021 - Topics in Cognitive Science 13 (1):206-223.
    McRae, Brown and Elman argue against the view that events are structured as frequently‐occurring sequences of world stimuli. They underline the importance of temporal structure defining event types and advance a more complex temporal structure, which allows for some variance in the component elements.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Solving the Black Box Problem: A Normative Framework for Explainable Artificial Intelligence.Carlos Zednik - 2019 - Philosophy and Technology 34 (2):265-288.
    Many of the computing systems programmed using Machine Learning are opaque: it is difficult to know why they do what they do or how they work. Explainable Artificial Intelligence aims to develop analytic techniques that render opaque computing systems transparent, but lacks a normative framework with which to evaluate these techniques’ explanatory successes. The aim of the present discussion is to develop such a framework, paying particular attention to different stakeholders’ distinct explanatory requirements. Building on an analysis of “opacity” from (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   56 citations  
  • Concepts, control, and context: A connectionist account of normal and disordered semantic cognition.Paul Hoffman, James L. McClelland & Matthew A. Lambon Ralph - 2018 - Psychological Review 125 (3):293-328.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Effects of prediction and contextual support on lexical processing: Prediction takes precedence.Trevor Brothers, Tamara Y. Swaab & Matthew J. Traxler - 2015 - Cognition 136:135-149.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Pigeons acquire multiple categories in parallel via associative learning: A parallel to human word learning?Edward A. Wasserman, Daniel I. Brooks & Bob McMurray - 2015 - Cognition 136 (C):99-122.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Dissociating multiple memory systems: Don't forsake the brain.Mark G. Packard - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):414-415.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Connectionist semantic systematicity.Stefan L. Frank, Willem F. G. Haselager & Iris van Rooij - 2009 - Cognition 110 (3):358-379.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: how important is starting small?Douglas L. T. Rohde & David C. Plaut - 1999 - Cognition 72 (1):67-109.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • Beyond Pavlovian classical conditioning.Beatrix T. Gardner & R. Allen Gardner - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):143-144.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Beyond modularity: Neural evidence for constructivist principles in development.Steven R. Quartz & Terrence J. Sejnowski - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):725-726.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Genes, development, and the “innate” structure of the mind.Timothy D. Johnston - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):721-722.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do you have to be right to redescribe?Susan Goldin-Meadow & Martha Wagner Alibali - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):718-719.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Psychoacoustic cues to emotion in speech prosody and music.Eduardo Coutinho & Nicola Dibben - 2013 - Cognition and Emotion 27 (4):658-684.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Are there interactive processes in speech perception?Lori L. Holt James L. McClelland, Daniel Mirman - 2006 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (8):363.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Lexical and Sublexical Units in Speech Perception.Ibrahima Giroux & Arnaud Rey - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (2):260-272.
    Saffran, Newport, and Aslin (1996a) found that human infants are sensitive to statistical regularities corresponding to lexical units when hearing an artificial spoken language. Two sorts of segmentation strategies have been proposed to account for this early word‐segmentation ability: bracketing strategies, in which infants are assumed to insert boundaries into continuous speech, and clustering strategies, in which infants are assumed to group certain speech sequences together into units (Swingley, 2005). In the present study, we test the predictions of two computational (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Similarity and Rules United: Similarity‐ and Rule‐Based Processing in a Single Neural Network.Tom Verguts & Wim Fias - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (2):243-259.
    A central controversy in cognitive science concerns the roles of rules versus similarity. To gain some leverage on this problem, we propose that rule‐ versus similarity‐based processes can be characterized as extremes in a multidimensional space that is composed of at least two dimensions: the number of features (Pothos, 2005) and the physical presence of features. The transition of similarity‐ to rule‐based processing is conceptualized as a transition in this space. To illustrate this, we show how a neural network model (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A Probabilistic Constraints Approach to Language Acquisition and Processing.Mark S. Seidenberg & Maryellen C. MacDonald - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):569-588.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Reduced Memory Representations for Music.Edward W. Large, Caroline Palmėr & Jordan B. Pollack - 1995 - Cognitive Science 19 (1):53-96.
    We address the problem of musical variation (identification of different musical sequences as variations) and its implications for mental representations of music. According to reductionist theories, listeners judge the structural importance of musical events while forming mental representations. These judgments may result from the production of reduced memory representations that retain only the musical gist. In a study of improvised music performance, pianists produced variations on melodies. Analyses of the musical events retained across variations provided support for the reductionist account (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Connectionist and Memory‐Array Models of Artificial Grammar Learning.Zoltan Dienes - 1992 - Cognitive Science 16 (1):41-79.
    Subjects exposed to strings of letters generated by a finite state grammar can later classify grammatical and nongrammatical test strings, even though they cannot adequately say what the rules of the grammar are (e.g., Reber, 1989). The MINERVA 2 (Hintzman, 1986) and Medin and Schaffer (1978) memory‐array models and a number of connectionist outoassociator models are tested against experimental data by deriving mainly parameter‐free predictions from the models of the rank order of classification difficulty of test strings. The importance of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   61 citations  
  • Structure and Content in Language Production: A Theory of Frame Constraints in Phonological Speech Errors.Gary S. Dell, Cornell Juliano & Anita Govindjee - 1993 - Cognitive Science 17 (2):149-195.
    Theories of language production propose that utterances are constructed by a mechanism that separates linguistic content from linguistic structure, Linguistic content is retrieved from the mental lexicon, and is then inserted into slots in linguistic structures or frames. Support for this kind of model at the phonological level comes from patterns of phonological speech errors. W present an alternative account of these patterns using a connectionist or parallel distributed proceesing (PDP) model that learns to produce sequences of phonological features. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • Explaining systematicity.Kenneth Aizawa - 1997 - Mind and Language 12 (2):115-36.
    Despite the considerable attention that the systematicity argument has enjoyed, it is worthwhile examining the argument within the context of similar explanatory arguments from the history of science. This kind of analysis helps show that Connectionism, qua Connectionism, really does not have an explanation of systematicity. Second, and more surprisingly, one finds that the systematicity argument sets such a high explanatory standard that not even Classicism can explain the systematicity of thought.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Spatiotemporal recursive hyperspheric classification with an application to dynamic gesture recognition.Salyer B. Reed, Tyson R. C. Reed & Sergiu M. Dascalu - 2019 - Artificial Intelligence 270 (C):41-66.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning a Generative Probabilistic Grammar of Experience: A Process‐Level Model of Language Acquisition.Oren Kolodny, Arnon Lotem & Shimon Edelman - 2015 - Cognitive Science 39 (2):227-267.
    We introduce a set of biologically and computationally motivated design choices for modeling the learning of language, or of other types of sequential, hierarchically structured experience and behavior, and describe an implemented system that conforms to these choices and is capable of unsupervised learning from raw natural‐language corpora. Given a stream of linguistic input, our model incrementally learns a grammar that captures its statistical patterns, which can then be used to parse or generate new data. The grammar constructed in this (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Can procedural learning be equated with unconscious learning or rule-based learning?Zoe Kourtzi, Lindsay M. Oliver & Mark A. Gluck - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):408-409.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learnability and Semantic Universals.Shane Steinert-Threlkeld & Jakub Szymanik - forthcoming - Semantics and Pragmatics.
    One of the great successes of the application of generalized quantifiers to natural language has been the ability to formulate robust semantic universals. When such a universal is attested, the question arises as to the source of the universal. In this paper, we explore the hypothesis that many semantic universals arise because expressions satisfying the universal are easier to learn than those that do not. While the idea that learnability explains universals is not new, explicit accounts of learning that can (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • When learning goes beyond statistics: Infants represent visual sequences in terms of chunks.Lauren K. Slone & Scott P. Johnson - 2018 - Cognition 178 (C):92-102.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Anatomy of a decision: Striato-orbitofrontal interactions in reinforcement learning, decision making, and reversal.Michael J. Frank & Eric D. Claus - 2006 - Psychological Review 113 (2):300-326.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Serial order learning of subliminal visual stimuli: evidence of multistage learning.Kaede Kido & Shogo Makioka - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Representational redescription and cognitive architectures.Antonella Carassa & Maurizio Tirassa - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):711-712.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Implicit practical learning.Elizabeth Ennen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):404-405.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning Orthographic Structure With Sequential Generative Neural Networks.Alberto Testolin, Ivilin Stoianov, Alessandro Sperduti & Marco Zorzi - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (3):579-606.
    Learning the structure of event sequences is a ubiquitous problem in cognition and particularly in language. One possible solution is to learn a probabilistic generative model of sequences that allows making predictions about upcoming events. Though appealing from a neurobiological standpoint, this approach is typically not pursued in connectionist modeling. Here, we investigated a sequential version of the restricted Boltzmann machine, a stochastic recurrent neural network that extracts high-order structure from sensory data through unsupervised generative learning and can encode contextual (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modelling unsupervised online-learning of artificial grammars: Linking implicit and statistical learning.Martin A. Rohrmeier & Ian Cross - 2014 - Consciousness and Cognition 27 (C):155-167.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge.Rebecca L. Gomez & LouAnn Gerken - 1999 - Cognition 70 (2):109-135.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   53 citations