- From pauses to clauses: Prosody facilitates learning of syntactic constituency.Kara Hawthorne & LouAnn Gerken - 2014 - Cognition 133 (2):420-428.details
|
|
Distributional Information: A Powerful Cue for Acquiring Syntactic Categories.Martin Redington, Nick Chater & Steven Finch - 1998 - Cognitive Science 22 (4):425-469.details
|
|
On the autonomy of language and gesture: evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language.Laura A. Petitto - 1987 - Cognition 27 (1):1-52.details
|
|
Granularity and the acquisition of grammatical gender: How order-of-acquisition affects what gets learned.Inbal Arnon & Michael Ramscar - 2012 - Cognition 122 (3):292-305.details
|
|
Cognitive science in the era of artificial intelligence: A roadmap for reverse-engineering the infant language-learner.Emmanuel Dupoux - 2018 - Cognition 173 (C):43-59.details
|
|
Probing the cognitive representation of musical time: Structural constraints on the perception of timing perturbations.Bruno H. Repp - 1992 - Cognition 44 (3):241-281.details
|
|
Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge.Rebecca L. Gomez & LouAnn Gerken - 1999 - Cognition 70 (2):109-135.details
|
|
Words in a sea of sounds: the output of infant statistical learning.Jenny R. Saffran - 2001 - Cognition 81 (2):149-169.details
|
|
Context Effects on Musical Chord Categorization: Different Forms of Top‐Down Feedback in Speech and Music?Bob McMurray, Joel L. Dennhardt & Andrew Struck-Marcell - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (5):893-920.details
|
|
A precursor of language acquisition in young infants.Jacques Mehler, Peter Jusczyk, Ghislaine Lambertz, Nilofar Halsted, Josiane Bertoncini & Claudine Amiel-Tison - 1988 - Cognition 29 (2):143-178.details
|
|
A perceptually-based model of children's earliest productions.Catharine H. Echols - 1993 - Cognition 46 (3):245-296.details
|
|
Bootstrapping the lexicon: a computational model of infant speech segmentation.Eleanor Olds Batchelder - 2002 - Cognition 83 (2):167-206.details
|
|
Native-language recognition abilities in 4-month-old infants from monolingual and bilingual environments.Laura Bosch & Núria Sebastián-Gallés - 1997 - Cognition 65 (1):33-69.details
|
|
Is infant-directed speech interesting because it is surprising? – Linking properties of IDS to statistical learning and attention at the prosodic level.Okko Räsänen, Sofoklis Kakouros & Melanie Soderstrom - 2018 - Cognition 178 (C):193-206.details
|
|
How much does prosody help word segmentation? A simulation study on infant-directed speech.Bogdan Ludusan, Alejandrina Cristia, Reiko Mazuka & Emmanuel Dupoux - 2022 - Cognition 219 (C):104961.details
|
|
A computational model to investigate assumptions in the headturn preference procedure.Christina Bergmann, Louis ten Bosch, Paula Fikkert & Lou Boves - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.details
|
|
A change is afoot: emergentist thinking in language acquisition.George Hollich, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, Michael L. Tucker & R. M. Golinkoff - 2000 - In P. B. Andersen, Claus Emmeche, N. O. Finnemann & P. V. Christiansen (eds.), Downward Causation. Aarhus, Denmark: University of Aarhus Press.details
|
|
Seeking the neurobiological bases of speech perception.Joanne L. Miller & Peter W. Jusczyk - 1989 - Cognition 33 (1-2):111-137.details
|
|
Evolution, selection, and cognition: From learning to parameter setting in biology and in the study of language.Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini - 1989 - Cognition 31 (1):1-44.details
|
|
Does sentential prosody help infants organize and remember speech information?Denise R. Mandel, Peter W. Jusczyk & Deborah G. Kemler Nelson - 1994 - Cognition 53 (2):155-180.details
|
|
Sensitivity to discontinuous dependencies in language learners: evidence for limitations in processing space.L. Santelmann - 1998 - Cognition 69 (2):105-134.details
|
|
Learning a Phonological Contrast Modulates the Auditory Grouping of Rhythm.H. Henny Yeung, Anjali Bhatara & Thierry Nazzi - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (6):2000-2020.details
|
|
The developmental course of lexical tone perception in the first year of life.Karen Mattock, Monika Molnar, Linda Polka & Denis Burnham - 2008 - Cognition 106 (3):1367-1381.details
|
|
When prosody fails to cue syntactic structure: 9-month-olds' sensitivity to phonological versus syntactic phrases.LouAnn Gerken, Peter W. Jusczyk & Denise R. Mandel - 1994 - Cognition 51 (3):237-265.details
|
|
Evidence of ordinal position encoding of sequences extracted from continuous speech.Ana Fló - 2021 - Cognition 213 (C):104646.details
|
|
“The tiger is hitting! the duck too!” 3-year-olds can use prosodic information to constrain their interpretation of ellipsis.Letícia Kolberg, Alex de Carvalho, Mireille Babineau, Naomi Havron, Anne-Caroline Fiévet, Bernadete Abaurre & Anne Christophe - 2021 - Cognition 213 (C):104626.details
|
|
Phrasal prosody constrains syntactic analysis in toddlers.Alex de Carvalho, Isabelle Dautriche, Isabelle Lin & Anne Christophe - 2017 - Cognition 163 (C):67-79.details
|
|
Infant memory for musical experiences.Jenny R. Saffran, Michelle M. Loman & Rachel R. W. Robertson - 2000 - Cognition 77 (1):B15-B23.details
|
|
Structural complexity and the time course of grammatical development.Robert Frank - 1998 - Cognition 66 (3):249-301.details
|
|
Early Prosodic Acquisition in Bilingual Infants: The Case of the Perceptual Trochaic Bias.Ranka Bijeljac-Babic, Barbara Höhle & Thierry Nazzi - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.details
|
|