Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. On panspatial theories of brain and behavior.Ernest Greene - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):503-503.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Considerations in evaluating the cognitive mapping theory of hippocampal function.Leonard E. Jarrard - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):509-509.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The cognitive map as a hippocampus.John O'Keefe & Lynn Nadel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):520-533.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   62 citations  
  • Mental maps, mental images, and intuitions about space.Steven Pinker - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):512-512.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Cognition, memory, and the hippocampus.Garth J. Thomas - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):515-517.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • “Model systems” versus “neuroethological” approach to hippocampal function.Richard F. Thompson, Paul R. Solomon & Donald J. Weisz - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):517-518.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive maps: dimensionality and development.J. Jacques Vonèche - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):519-520.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Précis of O'Keefe & Nadel's The hippocampus as a cognitive map.John O'Keefe & Lynn Nadel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):487-494.
    Theories of spatial cognition are derived from many sources. Psychologists are concerned with determining the features of the mind which, in combination with external inputs, produce our spatialized experience. A review of philosophical and other approaches has convinced us that the brain must come equipped to impose a three-dimensional Euclidean framework on experience – our analysis suggests that object re-identification may require such a framework. We identify this absolute, nonegocentric, spatial framework with a specific neural system centered in the hippocampus.A (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   178 citations  
  • The discovery of the artificial: some protocybernetic developments 1930-1940.Roberto Cordeschi - 1991 - Artificial Intelligence and Society 5 (3):218-238.
    In this paper I start from a definition of “culture of the artificial” which might be stated by referring to the background of philosophical, methodological, pragmatical assumptions which characterizes the development of the information processing analysis of mental processes and of some trends in contemporary cognitive science: in a word, the development of AI as a candidate science of mind. The aim of this paper is to show how (with which plausibility and limitations) the discovery of the mentioned background might (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Computational mechanisms underlying the dynamics of physical and cognitive fatigue.Julian Matthews, M. Andrea Pisauro, Mindaugas Jurgelis, Tanja Müller, Eliana Vassena, Trevor T.-J. Chong & Matthew A. J. Apps - 2023 - Cognition 240 (C):105603.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Finish what you started : 2-year-olds motivated by a preference for completing others' unfinished actions in instrumental helping contexts.John Michael, Alexander Green, Barbora Siposova, Keith Jensen & Sotaro Kita - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (6):e13160.
    A considerable body of research has documented the emergence of what appears to be instrumental helping behavior in early childhood. The current study tested the hypothesis that one basic psychological mechanism motivating this behavior is a preference for completing unfinished actions. To test this, a paradigm was implemented in which 2-year-olds (n = 34, 16 female/18 male, mostly White middle-class children) could continue an adult’s action when the adult no longer wanted to complete the action. The results showed that children (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive performance is enhanced if one knows when the task will end.Maayan Katzir, Aviv Emanuel & Nira Liberman - 2020 - Cognition 197 (C):104189.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Finish What you Started: 2‐Year‐Olds Motivated by a Preference for Completing Others’ Unfinished Actions in Instrumental Helping Contexts.John Michael, Alexander Green, Barbora Siposova, Keith Jensen & Sotaro Kita - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (6):e13160.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 6, June 2022.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “My Goal Is to Lose 2.923 kg!”—Efficacy of Precise Versus Round Goals for Body Weight Reduction.Marie-Lena Frech, Malte Friese & David D. Loschelder - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Overweight individuals often struggle to lose weight. While previous studies established goal setting as an effective strategy for weight loss, little is known about the effects of numeric goal precision. The present research investigated whether and how the precision of weight loss goals—the number of trailing zeros—impacts a goal’s effectiveness. In two preregistered, longitudinal experiments, we contrasted competing predictions as to whether precise or round goals are more effective compared to a waiting control condition. In Experiment 1, participants in the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Missing Link Between Memory and Reinforcement Learning.Christian Balkenius, Trond A. Tjøstheim, Birger Johansson, Annika Wallin & Peter Gärdenfors - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Reinforcement learning systems usually assume that a value function is defined over all states that can immediately give the value of a particular state or action. These values are used by a selection mechanism to decide which action to take. In contrast, when humans and animals make decisions, they collect evidence for different alternatives over time and take action only when sufficient evidence has been accumulated. We have previously developed a model of memory processing that includes semantic, episodic and working (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Getting Everyone Onboard: Framing Collective Goal Progress Broadens Participation in Collective Marketing Campaigns.Yaeeun Kim & Crystal Reeck - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Perceived Impact as the Underpinning Mechanism of the End-Spurt and U-Shape Pacing Patterns.Aviv Emanuel - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • How Focusing on Superordinate Goals Motivates Broad, Long-Term Goal Pursuit: A Theoretical Perspective.Bettina Höchli, Adrian Brügger & Claude Messner - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Goal-setting theory states that challenging, specific, and concrete goals (i.e., subordinate goals) are powerful motivators and boost performance in goal pursuit more than vague or abstract goals (i.e., superordinate goals). Goal-setting theory predominantly focuses on single, short-term goals and less on broad, long-term challenges. This review article extends goal-setting theory and argues that superordinate goals also fulfill a crucial role in motivating behavior, particularly when addressing broad, long-term challenges. The purpose of this article is to show theoretically that people pursue (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The climate change problem: promoting motivation for change when the map is not the territory.Idit Shalev - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Models of Temporal Discounting 1937–2000: An Interdisciplinary Exchange between Economics and Psychology.Till Grüne-Yanoff - 2015 - Science in Context 28 (4):675-713.
    ArgumentToday's models of temporal discounting are the result of multiple interdisciplinary exchanges between psychology and economics. Although these exchanges did not result in an integrated discipline, they had important effects on all disciplines involved. The paper describes these exchanges from the 1930s onwards, focusing on two episodes in particular: an attempted synthesis by psychiatrist George Ainslie and others in the 1970s; and the attempted application of this new discounting model by a generation of economists and psychologists in the 1980s, which (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • The discovery of the artificial. Some protocybernetic developments 1930–1940.Roberto Cordeschi - 1991 - AI and Society 5 (3):218-238.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Curiosity and time: from not knowing to almost knowing.Marret K. Noordewier & Eric van Dijk - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (3).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Differential recall of problem names and clues as a function of problem solution or nonsolution.Robert A. Bottenberg, Melvin H. Marx & Edward J. Pavur - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 7 (5):445-448.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Anatomical units in psychology.Holger Ursin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):518-518.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is hippocampal rhythmical slow activity specifically related to movement through space?C. M. Vanderwolf - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):518-519.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The hippocampus and informational salience.John W. Moore - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):510-511.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Time: a fourth dimension for the hippocampal cognitive map.Arthur J. Nonneman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):511-511.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Inner and outer space: the neuroanatomical bases of spatially organized behaviors.David S. Olton - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):511-512.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On O'Keefe, Nadel, space and brain.James B. Ranck - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):513-514.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The hippocampus, space, and human amnesia.Larry R. Squire - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):514-515.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The anatomy of a cognitive map.L. W. Swanson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):515-515.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the nature of cognitive maps.Roger M. Downs - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):499-500.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The hippocampus and operant behavior.Paul Ellen - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):500-501.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Spatial mapping only a special case of hippocampal function.Jeffrey A. Gray - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):501-503.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Cortical areas involved in spatial function.H. Hécaen - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):503-504.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Hippocampal function: logic, logic, and more logic.Richard Hirsh & Joel Krajden - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):504-505.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Waves and cells, maps and memories, space and time.J. Eric Holmes - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):505-506.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Lost maps and memories.James A. Horel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):506-507.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Hippocampal lesions and Intermittent reinforcement.Robert L. Isaacson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):507-507.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What is a cognitive map?Ray Jackendoff - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):507-509.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Hippocampus and memory.Raymond P. Kesner - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):509-510.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Hippocampus, memory and movement.Abram Amsel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):494-495.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Selective activation of hippocampal neurons.Theodore W. Berger - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):495-496.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Behavioral analysis of the hippocampal syndrome.D. Caroline Blanchard & Robert J. Blanchard - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):496-496.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • O'Keefe & Nadel's three-stage model for hippocampal representation of space.T. V. P. Bliss - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):496-497.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The “neuroethological revolution” in unit studies.Jan Bureš - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):497-498.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The hippocampus and its apparent migration to the parietal lobe.Robert J. Douglas - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):498-499.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark