Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Téléologie et fonctions en biologie. Une approche non causale des explications téléofonctionnelles.Alberto Molina Pérez - 2017 - Dissertation, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
    This dissertation focuses on teleology and functions in biology. More precisely, it focuses on the scientific legitimacy of teleofunctional attributions and explanations in biology. It belongs to a multi-faceted debate that can be traced back to at least the 1970s. One aspect of the debate concerns the naturalization of functions. Most authors try to reduce, translate or explain functions and teleology in terms of efficient causes so that they find their place in the framework of the natural sciences. Our approach (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Do Apes Read Minds?: Toward a New Folk Psychology.Kristin Andrews - 2012 - MIT Press.
    Andrews argues for a pluralistic folk psychology that employs different kinds of practices and different kinds of cognitive tools (including personality trait attribution, stereotype activation, inductive reasoning about past behavior, and ...
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   94 citations  
  • Infants track action goals within and across agents.Jennifer Sootsman Buresh & Amanda L. Woodward - 2007 - Cognition 104 (2):287-314.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Primacy of Information About Means Selection Over Outcome Selection in Goal Attribution by Infants.Stephan Verschoor & Szilvia Biro - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (4):714-725.
    It has been shown that, when observing an action, infants can rely on either outcome selection information (i.e., actions that express a choice between potential outcomes) or means selection information (i.e., actions that are causally efficient toward the outcome) in their goal attribution. However, no research has investigated the relationship between these two types of information when they are present simultaneously. In an experiment that addressed this question directly, we found that when outcome selection information could disambiguate the goal of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • (1 other version)Direct perception in the intersubjective context.Shaun Gallagher - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (2):535-543.
    This paper, in opposition to the standard theories of social cognition found in psychology and cognitive science, defends the idea that direct perception plays an important role in social cognition. The two dominant theories, theory theory and simulation theory , both posit something more than a perceptual element as necessary for our ability to understand others, i.e., to “mindread” or “mentalize.” In contrast, certain phenomenological approaches depend heavily on the concept of perception and the idea that we have a direct (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   257 citations  
  • Inference or interaction: Social cognition without precursors.Shaun Gallagher - 2008 - Philosophical Explorations 11 (3):163 – 174.
    In this paper I defend interaction theory (IT) as an alternative to both theory theory (TT) and simulation theory (ST). IT opposes the basic suppositions that both TT and ST depend upon. I argue that the various capacities for primary and secondary intersubjectivity found in infancy and early childhood should not be thought of as precursors to later developing capacities for using folk psychology or simulation routines. They are not replaced or displaced by such capacities in adulthood, but rather continue (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   109 citations  
  • Do I get what you get? Learning about the effects of self-performed and observed actions in infancy.Birgit Elsner & Gisa Aschersleben - 2003 - Consciousness and Cognition 12 (4):732-751.
    The present study investigated whether infants learn the effects of other persons' actions like they do for their own actions, and whether infants transfer observed action-effect relations to their own actions. Nine-, 12-, 15- and 18-month-olds explored an object that allowed two actions, and that produced a certain salient effect after each action. In a self-exploration group, infants explored the object directly, whereas in two observation groups, infants first watched an adult model acting on the object and obtaining a certain (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Core morality? Or merely core agents and social beings? A response to Spelke's what babies know.J. Kiley Hamlin - 2023 - Mind and Language 38 (5):1323-1335.
    Spelke'sWhat babies knowdescribes the remarkably sophisticated mental lives of infants through the theoretical framework of core knowledge. To Spelke, young infants possess six independent core domains, two of which allow them to reason about the social world: the core agent and the core social being systems. Critically, Spelke argues that these core systems fail to communicate prior to 10 months, resulting in an inability to understand social goals. In this commentary, I review evidence that, contrary to Spelke's claims, young infants (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Infants’ motor simulation of observed actions is modulated by the visibility of the actor’s body.Ty W. Boyer, Samuel M. Harding & Bennett I. Bertenthal - 2017 - Cognition 164 (C):107-115.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • (1 other version)Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions. [REVIEW]Stephen Darling, Tim Valentine, Stephen R. Mitroff, Brian J. Scholl, Karen Wynn, Jessica A. Sommerville, Amanda L. Woodward, Amy Needham, Jyrki Tuomainen & Tobias S. Andersen - 2005 - Cognition 96 (1):B1-B11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   46 citations  
  • (1 other version)Can an agent’s false belief be corrected by an appropriate communication? Psychological reasoning in 18-month-old infants.Hyun-joo Song, Kristine H. Onishi, Renée Baillargeon & Cynthia Fisher - 2008 - Cognition 109 (3):295-315.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • (1 other version)Infants’ understanding of actions performed by mechanical devices.Ty W. Boyer, J. Samantha Pan & Bennett I. Bertenthal - 2011 - Cognition 121 (1):1-11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Ontogeny, phylogeny, and the relational reinterpretation hypothesis.Elizabeth V. Hallinan & Valerie A. Kuhlmeier - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (2):138-139.
    If our knowledge of human cognition were based solely on research with participants younger than the age of 2 years, there would be no basis for the relational reinterpretation hypothesis, and Darwin's continuity theory would be safe as houses. Because many of the shortcomings cited apply to human infants, we propose how a consideration of cognitive development would inform the relational reinterpretation hypothesis.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Magically deceptive biological motion—the French Drop Sleight.Flip Phillips, Michael B. Natter & Eric J. L. Egan - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Infants attribute goals even to biomechanically impossible actions.Victoria Southgate, Mark H. Johnson & Gergely Csibra - 2008 - Cognition 107 (3):1059-1069.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Sharing and Ascribing Goals.Pierre Jacob - 2012 - Mind and Language 27 (2):200-227.
    This paper assesses the scope and limits of a widely influential model of goal-ascription by human infants: the shared-intentionality model. It derives much of its appeal from its ability to integrate behavioral evidence from developmental psychology with cognitive neuroscientific evidence about the role of mirror neuron activity in non-human primates. The central question raised by this model is whether sharing a goal with an agent is necessary and sufficient for ascribing it to that agent. I argue that advocates of the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • (1 other version)Can an agent's false belief be corrected by an appropriate communication? Psychological reasoning in 18-month-old infants.Cynthia Fisher Hyun-joo Song, Kristine H. Onishi, Renée Baillargeon - 2008 - Cognition 109 (3):295.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy.Jessica A. Sommerville & Amanda L. Woodward - 2005 - Cognition 95 (1):1-30.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  • Understanding infants' understanding of intentions: Two problems of interpretation.Jessica Heineman-Pieper & Amanda Woodward - 2003 - Consciousness and Cognition 12 (4):770-772.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions?Yuyan Luo & Renée Baillargeon - 2007 - Cognition 105 (3):489-512.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   48 citations  
  • (1 other version)Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions.Jessica A. Sommerville, Amanda L. Woodward & Amy Needham - 2005 - Cognition 96 (1):B1-B11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   93 citations  
  • Intentions as Complex Entities.Marco Mazzone - 2011 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 2 (4):767-783.
    In the philosophical and cognitive literature, the word ‘intention’ has been used with a variety of meanings which occasionally have been explicitly distinguished. I claim that an important cause of this polysemy is the fact that intentions are complex entities, endowed with an internal structure, and that sometimes different theories in the field are erroneously presented as if they were in conflict with each other, while they in fact just focus on different aspects of the phenomenon. The debate between Gallese’s (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The developing cognitive substrate of sequential action control in 9- to 12-month-olds: Evidence for concurrent activation models. [REVIEW]S. A. Verschoor, M. Paulus, M. Spapé, S. Biro & B. Hommel - 2015 - Cognition 138 (C):64-78.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Infants’ understanding of everyday social interactions: A dual process account.Gustaf Gredebäck & Annika Melinder - 2010 - Cognition 114 (2):197-206.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Infants’ Goal Prediction for Simple Action Events: The Role of Experience and Agency Cues.Birgit Elsner & Maurits Adam - 2021 - Topics in Cognitive Science 13 (1):45-62.
    Looking times and gaze behavior indicate that infants can predict the goal state of an observed simple action event (e.g., object‐directed grasping) already in the first year of life. The present paper mainly focuses on infants’ predictive gaze‐shifts toward the goal of an ongoing action. For this, infants need to generate a forward model of the to‐be‐obtained goal state and to disengage their gaze from the moving agent at a time when information about the action event is still incomplete. By (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Reasoning about ‘irrational’ actions: When intentional movements cannot be explained, the movements themselves are seen as the goal.Adena Schachner & Susan Carey - 2013 - Cognition 129 (2):309-327.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Infants’ Understanding of Object-Directed Action: An Interdisciplinary Synthesis.Scott J. Robson & Valerie A. Kuhlmeier - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation