Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Age-Related Interference between the Selection of Input-Output Modality Mappings and Postural Control—a Pilot Study.Christine Stelzel, Gesche Schauenburg, Michael A. Rapp, Stephan Heinzel & Urs Granacher - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Movement-Image Compatibility Effect: Embodiment Theory Interpretations of Motor Resonance With Digitized Photographs, Drawings, and Paintings.Mark-Oliver Casper, John A. Nyakatura, Anja Pawel, Christina B. Reimer, Torsten Schubert & Marion Lauschke - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:326863.
    To evoke the impression of movement in the “immobile” image is one of the central motivations of the visual art, and the activating effect of images has been discussed in art psychology already some hundred years ago. However, this topic has up to now been largely neglected by the researchers in cognitive psychology and neuroscience. This study investigates – from an interdisciplinary perspective – the formation of lateralised instances of motion when an observer perceives movement in an image. A first (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • SOAR as a world view, not a theory.Earl Hunt & R. Duncan Luce - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):447-448.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Problem spaces, language and connectionism: Issues for cognition.Patrick Suppes - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):457-458.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pseudo‐mechanistic Explanations in Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience.Bernhard Hommel - 2020 - Topics in Cognitive Science 12 (4):1294-1305.
    Pseudo‐mechanistic explanations in psychology and cognitive neuroscienceThis paper focuses on the level of systems/cognitive neuroscience. It argues that the great majority of explanations in psychology and cognitive neuroscience is “pseudo‐mechanistic.” On the basis of various case studies, Hommel argues that cognitive neuroscience should move beyond what he calls an “Aristotelian phase” to become a mature “Galilean” science seeking to discover actual mechanisms of cognitive phenomena.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Task representation in individual and joint settings.Wolfgang Prinz - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • The SNARC effect: an instance of the Simon effect?Daniela Mapelli, Elena Rusconi & Carlo Umiltà - 2003 - Cognition 88 (3):B1-B10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Is Unified theories of cognition good strategy?Nico H. Frijda & Jan Elshout - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):445-446.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Unifying congnition: Has it all been put together?John A. Michon - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):450-451.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On models and mechanisms.William R. Uttal - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):459-460.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Précis of Unified theories of cognition.Allen Newell - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):425-437.
    The book presents the case that cognitive science should turn its attention to developing theories of human cognition that cover the full range of human perceptual, cognitive, and action phenomena. Cognitive science has now produced a massive number of high-quality regularities with many microtheories that reveal important mechanisms. The need for integration is pressing and will continue to increase. Equally important, cognitive science now has the theoretical concepts and tools to support serious attempts at unified theories. The argument is made (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Unconscious modulation of the conscious experience of voluntary control.Katrin Linser & Thomas Goschke - 2007 - Cognition 104 (3):459-475.
    How does the brain generate our experience of being in control over our actions and their effects? Here, we argue that the perception of events as self-caused emerges from a comparison between anticipated and actual action-effects: if the representation of an event that follows an action is activated before the action, the event is experienced as caused by one’s own action, whereas in the case of a mismatch it will be attributed to an external cause rather than to the self. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Are "implicit" attitudes unconscious?Bertram Gawronski, Wilhelm Hofmann & Christopher J. Wilbur - 2006 - Consciousness and Cognition 15 (3):485-499.
    A widespread assumption in recent research on attitudes is that self-reported evaluations reflect conscious attitudes, whereas indirectly assessed evaluations reflect unconscious attitudes. The present article reviews the available evidence regarding unconscious features of indirectly assessed “implicit” attitudes. Distinguishing between three different aspects of attitudes, we conclude that people sometimes lack conscious awareness of the origin of their attitudes, but that lack of source awareness is not a distinguishing feature of indirectly assessed versus self-reported attitudes, there is no evidence that people (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   45 citations  
  • Expectancy to Eat Modulates Cognitive Control and Attention Toward Irrelevant Food and Non-food Images in Healthy Starving Individuals. A Behavioral Study.Sami Schiff, Giulia Testa, Maria Luisa Rusconi, Paolo Angeli & Daniela Mapelli - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    It is thought that just as hunger itself, the expectancy to eat impacts attention and cognitive control toward food stimuli, but this theory has not been extensively explored at a behavioral level. In order to study the effect of expectancy to eat on attentional and cognitive control mechanisms, 63 healthy fasting participants were presented with an affective priming spatial compatibility Simon task that included both food and object distracters. The participants were randomly assigned to two groups: an “immediate expectancy” group (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Designing Smart Objects to Support Affording Situations: Exploiting Affordance Through an Understanding of Forms of Engagement.Chris Baber - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Basics for sensorimotor information processing: some implications for learning.Franck Vidal, Cã©Dric Meckler & Thierry Hasbroucq - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • How Task Goals Mediate the Interplay between Perception and Action.Pascal Haazebroek, Saskia van Dantzig & Bernhard Hommel - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Imaging When Acting: Picture but Not Word Cues Induce Action-Related Biases of Visual Attention.Agnieszka Wykowska, Bernhard Hommel & Anna Schubö - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Unified theories and theories that mimic each other's predictions.James T. Townsend - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):458-459.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Common valence coding in action and evaluation: Affective blindness towards response-compatible stimuli.Andreas B. Eder & Karl Christoph Klauer - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (6):1297-1322.
    A common coding account of bidirectional evaluation–behaviour interactions proposes that evaluative attributes of stimuli and responses are coded in a common representational format. This assumption was tested in two experiments that required evaluations of positive and negative stimuli during the generation of a positively or negatively charged motor response. The results of both experiments revealed a reduced evaluative sensitivity (d′) towards response-compatible stimulus valences. This action–valence blindness supports the notion of a common valence coding in action and evaluation.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Indexes for action.Joëlle Proust - 1999 - Revue Internationale de Philosophie 1999 (3):321-345.
    This articles examines three ways in which the connection between semantic and pragmatic representations of a single action can be tightened up in order to remedy the puzzle of deviant causation. A first move consists in making the feedback process, i.e. the dynamics of the relationship between both representational components, a central element in the definition of an action. A second step brings in the action-effect principle, emphasizing the teleological relation of each pragmatic representation type with its external effects. A (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • (1 other version)Looking before you leap: A theory of motivated control of action.Peter F. Liddle Elizabeth B. Liddle, Gaia Scerif, Christopher P. Hollis, Martin J. Batty, Madeleine J. Groom, Mario Liotti - 2009 - Cognition 112 (1):141.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Self-regulatory strategy and executive control : implementation intentions modulate task switching and Simon task performance.Anna-Lisa Cohen, Ute C. Bayer, Alexander Jaudas & Peter M. Gollwitzer - unknown
    Two tasks where failures of cognitive control are especially prevalent are task switching and spatial Simon task paradigms. Both tasks require considerable strategic control for the participant to avoid the costs associated with switching tasks (task-switching paradigm) and to minimize the influence of spatial location (Simon task). In the current study, we assessed whether the use of a self-regulatory strategy known as “implementation intentions” would have any beneficial effects on performance in each of these task domains. Forming an implementation intention (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Perceptual Similarity: Insights From Crossmodal Correspondences.Nicola Di Stefano & Charles Spence - 2024 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 15 (3):997-1026.
    Perceptual similarity is one of the most fiercely debated topics in the philosophy and psychology of perception. The documented history of the issue spans all the way from Plato – who regarded similarity as a key factor for human perceptual experience and cognition – through to contemporary psychologists – who have tried to determine whether, and if so, how similarity relationships can be established between stimuli both within and across the senses. Recent research on cross-sensory associations, otherwise known as crossmodal (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The joint Simon effect depends on perceived agency, but not intentionality, of the alternative action.Anna Stenzel, Thomas Dolk, Lorenza S. Colzato, Roberta Sellaro, Bernhard Hommel & Roman Liepelt - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8:96464.
    A co-actor’s intentionality has been suggested to be a key modulating factor for joint action effects like the joint Simon effect (JSE). However, in previous studies intentionality has often been confounded with agency defined as perceiving the initiator of an action as being the causal source of the action. The aim of the present study was to disentangle the role of agency and intentionality as modulating factors of the JSE. In Experiment 1, participants performed a joint go/nogo Simon task next (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Does conflict help or hurt cognitive control? Initial evidence for an inverted U-shape relationship between perceived task difficulty and conflict adaptation.Henk van Steenbergen, Guido P. H. Band & Bernhard Hommel - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Uncovering the connection between artist and audience: Viewing painted brushstrokes evokes corresponding action representations in the observer.J. Eric T. Taylor, Jessica K. Witt & Phillip J. Grimaldi - 2012 - Cognition 125 (1):26-36.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Religion and action control: Faith-specific modulation of the Simon effect but not Stop-Signal performance.Bernhard Hommel, Lorenza S. Colzato, Claudia Scorolli, Anna M. Borghi & Wery P. M. van den Wildenberg - 2011 - Cognition 120 (2):177-185.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Re-membering cognition.Susan F. Chipman - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):441-442.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Affect and action: Towards an event-coding account.Tristan Lavender & Bernhard Hommel - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (6):1270-1296.
    Viewing emotion from an evolutionary perspective, researchers have argued that simple responses to affective stimuli can be triggered without mediation of cognitive processes. Indeed, findings suggest that positively and negatively valenced stimuli trigger approach and avoidance movements automatically. However, affective stimulus–response compatibility phenomena share so many central characteristics with nonaffective stimulus–response compatibility phenomena that one may doubt whether the underlying mechanisms differ. We suggest an “affectively enriched” version of the theory of event coding (TEC) that is able to account for (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Hypnotic suggestibility predicts the magnitude of the imaginative word blindness suggestion effect in a non-hypnotic context.Benjamin A. Parris & Zoltan Dienes - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (3):868-874.
    The present study investigated how the magnitude the word blindness suggestion effect on Stroop interference depended on hypnotic suggestibility when given as an imaginative suggestion and under conditions in which hypnosis was not mentioned. Hypnotic suggestibility is shown to be a significant predictor of the magnitude of the imaginative word blindness suggestion effect under these conditions. This is therefore the first study to show a linear relationship between the imaginative word blindness suggestion effect and hypnotic suggestibility across the whole hypnotizability (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Sensitivity of different measures of the visibility of masked primes: Influences of prime–response and prime–target relations.Shah Khalid, Peter König & Ulrich Ansorge - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4):1473-1488.
    Visual masking of primes lowers prime visibility but spares processing of primes as reflected in prime–target congruence and prime–response compatibility effects. However, the question is how to appropriately measure prime visibility. Here, we tested the influence of three procedural variables on prime visibility measures: prime–target similarity, prime–response similarity, and the variability of prime–response mappings. Our results show that a low prime–target similarity is a favorable condition for a prime visibility measure because it increases the sensitivity of this measure in comparison (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Codes and their vicissitudes.Bernhard Hommel, Jochen Müsseler, Gisa Aschersleben & Wolfgang Prinz - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (5):910-926.
    First, we discuss issues raised with respect to the Theory of Event Coding (TEC)'s scope, that is, its limitations and possible extensions. Then, we address the issue of specificity, that is, the widespread concern that TEC is too unspecified and, therefore, too vague in a number of important respects. Finally, we elaborate on our views about TEC's relations to other important frameworks and approaches in the field like stages models, ecological approaches, and the two-visual-pathways model. Footnotes1 We acknowledge the precedence (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Spatial-numerical associations of manual response codes are strongly asymmetrical.Melanie Richter & Peter Wühr - 2023 - Cognition 238 (C):105538.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neural Dynamics of Cognitive Control in Various Types of Incongruence.Liufang Xie, Bihua Cao, Zixia Li & Fuhong Li - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Understanding and Resolving Failures in Human-Robot Interaction: Literature Review and Model Development.Shanee Honig & Tal Oron-Gilad - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:351644.
    While substantial effort has been invested in making robots more reliable, experience demonstrates that robots operating in unstructured environments are often challenged by frequent failures. Despite this, robots have not yet reached a level of design that allows effective management of faulty or unexpected behavior by untrained users. To understand why this may be the case, an in-depth literature review was done to explore when people perceive and resolve robot failures, how robots communicate failure, how failures influence people's perceptions and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Where you are affects what you can easily imagine: Environmental geometry elicits sensorimotor interference in remote perspective taking.Bernhard E. Riecke & Timothy P. McNamara - 2017 - Cognition 169 (C):1-14.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Challenging Cognitive Control by Mirrored Stimuli in Working Memory Matching.Maria Wirth & Robert Gaschler - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unified cognitive theory is not comprehensive.P. C. Dodwell - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):443-445.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Choosing a unifying theory for cognitive development.Thomas R. Shultz - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):456-457.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Attentional influences on affective priming: Does categorisation influence spontaneous evaluations of multiply categorisable objects?Bertram Gawronski, William A. Cunningham, Etienne P. LeBel & Roland Deutsch - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (6):1008-1025.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • When more is less: Adding action effects to reduce crosstalk between concurrently performed tasks.Jonathan Schacherer & Eliot Hazeltine - 2023 - Cognition 230 (C):105318.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Affective and Semantic Representations of Valence: A Conceptual Framework.Oksana Itkes & Assaf Kron - 2019 - Emotion Review 11 (4):283-293.
    The current article discusses the distinction between affective valence—the degree to which an affective response represents pleasure or displeasure—and semantic valence, the degree to which an object or event is considered positive or negative. To date, measures that reflect positivity and negativity are usually placed under the same conceptual umbrella (e.g., valence, affective, emotional), with minimal distinction between the modes of valence they reflect. Recent work suggests that what might seem to reflect a monolithic structure of valence has at least (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Temporal expectancies and rhythmic cueing in touch: The influence of spatial attention.Alexander Jones - 2019 - Cognition 182 (C):140-150.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Dynamic field theory of movement preparation.Wolfram Erlhagen & Gregor Schöner - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (3):545-572.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Unimanual SNARC Effect: Hand Matters.Marianna Riello & Elena Rusconi - 2011 - Frontiers in Psychology 2.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Are Automatic Conceptual Cores the Gold Standard of Semantic Processing? The Context‐Dependence of Spatial Meaning in Grounded Congruency Effects.Lauren A. M. Lebois, Christine D. Wilson-Mendenhall & Lawrence W. Barsalou - 2015 - Cognitive Science 39 (8):1764-1801.
    According to grounded cognition, words whose semantics contain sensory-motor features activate sensory-motor simulations, which, in turn, interact with spatial responses to produce grounded congruency effects. Growing evidence shows these congruency effects do not always occur, suggesting instead that the grounded features in a word's meaning do not become active automatically across contexts. Researchers sometimes use this as evidence that concepts are not grounded, further concluding that grounded information is peripheral to the amodal cores of concepts. We first review broad evidence (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • Perceiving by proxy: Effect-based action control with unperceivable effects.Roland Pfister, Christina U. Pfeuffer & Wilfried Kunde - 2014 - Cognition 132 (3):251-261.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Representing others' actions: just like one's own?Natalie Sebanz, Günther Knoblich & Wolfgang Prinz - 2003 - Cognition 88 (3):B11-B21.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   101 citations  
  • What is Shared in Joint Action? Issues of Co-representation, Response Conflict, and Agent Identification.Dorit Wenke, Silke Atmaca, Antje Holländer, Roman Liepelt, Pamela Baess & Wolfgang Prinz - 2011 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 2 (2):147-172.
    When sharing a task with another person that requires turn taking, as in doubles games of table tennis, performance on the shared task is similar to performing the whole task alone. This has been taken to indicate that humans co-represent their partner’s task share, as if it were their own. Task co-representation allows prediction of the other’s responses when it is the other’s turn, and leads to response conflict in joint interference tasks. However, data from our lab cast doubt on (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations