Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. (1 other version)Explanation and Constructions: Response to Adger.Adele E. Goldberg - 2013 - Mind and Language 28 (4):479-491.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • But do we need universal grammar? Comment on Lidz et al.Adele E. Goldberg - 2004 - Cognition 94 (1):77-84.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Yes, we still need Universal Grammar.Jeffrey Lidz & Lila R. Gleitman - 2004 - Cognition 94 (1):85-93.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • What infants know about syntax but couldn't have learned: experimental evidence for syntactic structure at 18 months.Jeffrey Lidz, Sandra Waxman & Jennifer Freedman - 2003 - Cognition 89 (3):295-303.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • Comprehension of Argument Structure and Semantic Roles: Evidence from English-Learning Children and the Forced-Choice Pointing Paradigm.Claire H. Noble, Caroline F. Rowland & Julian M. Pine - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (5):963-982.
    Research using the intermodal preferential looking paradigm (IPLP) has consistently shown that English‐learning children aged 2 can associate transitive argument structure with causal events. However, studies using the same methodology investigating 2‐year‐old children’s knowledge of the conjoined agent intransitive and semantic role assignment have reported inconsistent findings. The aim of the present study was to establish at what age English‐learning children have verb‐general knowledge of both transitive and intransitive argument structure using a new method: the forced‐choice pointing paradigm. The results (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Predicted errors in children’s early sentence comprehension.Yael Gertner & Cynthia Fisher - 2012 - Cognition 124 (1):85-94.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • iMinerva: A Mathematical Model of Distributional Statistical Learning.Erik D. Thiessen & Philip I. Pavlik - 2013 - Cognitive Science 37 (2):310-343.
    Statistical learning refers to the ability to identify structure in the input based on its statistical properties. For many linguistic structures, the relevant statistical features are distributional: They are related to the frequency and variability of exemplars in the input. These distributional regularities have been suggested to play a role in many different aspects of language learning, including phonetic categories, using phonemic distinctions in word learning, and discovering non-adjacent relations. On the surface, these different aspects share few commonalities. Despite this, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Public language, private language, and subsymbolic theories of mind.Gabe Dupre - 2023 - Mind and Language 38 (2):394-412.
    Language has long been a problem‐case for subsymbolic theories of mind. The reason for this is obvious: Language seems essentially symbolic. However, recent work has developed a potential solution to this problem, arguing that linguistic symbols are public objects which augment a fundamentally subsymbolic mind, rather than components of cognitive symbol‐processing. I shall argue that this strategy cannot work, on the grounds that human language acquisition consists in projecting linguistic structure onto environmental entities, rather than extracting this structure from them.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Who “it” is influences what “it” does: Discourse effects on children's syntactic parsing.Yi Ting Huang & Zoe Ovans - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (1):e13076.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 1, January 2022.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Children's attitude problems: Bootstrapping verb meaning from syntax and pragmatics.Valentine Hacquard & Jeffrey Lidz - 2019 - Mind and Language 34 (1):73-96.
    How do children learn the meanings of propositional attitude verbs? We argue that children use information contained in both syntactic distribution and pragmatic function to zero in on the appropriate meanings. Specifically, we identify a potentially universal link between semantic subclasses of attitude verbs, their syntactic distribution and the kinds of indirect speech acts they can be used to perform. As a result, children can use the syntax as evidence about the meaning, which in turn constrains the kinds of pragmatic (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Mandarin learners use syntactic bootstrapping in verb acquisition.Joanne N. Lee & Letitia R. Naigles - 2008 - Cognition 106 (2):1028-1037.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Developmental effects in the online use of morphosyntactic cues in sentence processing: Evidence from Tagalog.Rowena Garcia, Gabriela Garrido Rodriguez & Evan Kidd - 2021 - Cognition 216 (C):104859.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Using instruments to understand argument structure: Evidence for gradient representation.Lilia Rissman, Kyle Rawlins & Barbara Landau - 2015 - Cognition 142 (C):266-290.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Subtle Implicit Language Facts Emerge from the Functions of Constructions.Adele E. Goldberg - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The role of language in building abstract, generalized conceptual representations of one- and two-place predicates: A comparison between adults and infants.Mohinish Shukla & Jill de Villiers - 2021 - Cognition 213 (C):104705.
    Theories of relations between language and conceptual development benefit from empirical evidence for concepts available in infancy, but such evidence is comparatively scarce. Here, we examine early representations of specific concepts, namely, sets of dynamic events corresponding either to predicates involving two variables with a reversible, asymmetric relation between them (such as the set of all events that correspond to a linguistic phrase like “a dog is pushing a car,”) or to comparatively simpler, one-variable predicates (such as the set of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Word learning in linguistic context: Processing and memory effects.Yi Ting Huang & Alison R. Arnold - 2016 - Cognition 156 (C):71-87.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The nature of generalization in language.Adele E. Goldberg - 2009 - Cognitive Linguistics 20 (1):93-127.
    This paper provides a concise overview of Constructions at Work (Goldberg 2006). The book aims to investigate the relevant levels of generalization in adult language, how and why generalizations are learned by children, and how to account for cross-linguistic generalizations.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Becoming syntactic.Franklin Chang, Gary S. Dell & Kathryn Bock - 2006 - Psychological Review 113 (2):234-272.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   124 citations  
  • Minimally innate ideas.Michele Merritt - unknown
    This project provides a detailed examination and critique of current philosophical, linguistic, and cognitive accounts of first language acquisition. In particular, I focus on the concept of "innate" and how it is embraced, marginally utilized, or abandoned altogether in efforts to describe the way that a child comes to be a competent user of a language. A central question that naturally falls out of this general inquiry is therefore what exactly is supposed to be "innate," according to various theories? Philosophically, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Lexical Disambiguation in Verb Learning: Evidence from the Conjoined-Subject Intransitive Frame in English and Mandarin Chinese.Sudha Arunachalam, Kristen Syrett & YongXiang Chen - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Easy Words: Reference Resolution in a Malevolent Referent World.Lila R. Gleitman & John C. Trueswell - 2018 - Topics in Cognitive Science 12 (1):22-47.
    Gleitman and Trueswell’s “The easy words” forms a pair with their earlier paper, “Hard words,” completing a circle in which the authors ask how “easy” words (e.g., concrete nouns) are learned. They take up the hypothesis of “cross‐situational learning,” and argue that accumulating observations actually hinders learning if the mechanism requires holding all exemplars in memory over time. They present an alternative hypothesis, “Propose but Verify,” wherein people use one‐trial learning to confirm or disconfirm their current hypothesis—a mechanism distinctly different (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • When we think about thinking: The acquisition of belief verbs.Anna Papafragou - 2007 - Cognition 105 (1):125.
    Mental-content verbs such as think, believe, imagine and hope seem to pose special problems for the young language learner. One possible explanation for these diYculties is that the concepts that these verbs express are hard to grasp and therefore their acquisition must await relevant conceptual development. According to a diVerent, perhaps complementary, proposal, a major contributor to the diYculty of these items lies with the informational requirements for identifying them from the contexts in which they appear. The experiments reported here (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Learning, Memory, and Syntactic Bootstrapping: A Meditation.Jeffrey Lidz - 2020 - Topics in Cognitive Science 12 (1):78-90.
    Lidz also ponders the theory of syntactic bootstrapping by asking why it is that learners are considered to retain little of extralinguistic environments (i.e., their observations) during word learning, while being able to retain detailed representations of linguistic context, for example, the multiple syntactic frames in which a verb appears. Lidz argues that learners value syntactic information over extralinguistic context from the beginning of learning, consistent with syntactic bootstrapping as a key device for verb learning.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Word frequency as a cue for identifying function words in infancy.Jean-Rémy Hochmann, Ansgar D. Endress & Jacques Mehler - 2010 - Cognition 115 (3):444-457.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Word frequency, function words and the second gavagai problem.Jean-Rémy Hochmann - 2013 - Cognition 128 (1):13-25.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • (1 other version)Explanation and Constructions: Response to Adger.Adele E. Goldberg - 2013 - Mind and Language 28 (4):479-491.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • (1 other version)Modeling Statistical Insensitivity: Sources of Suboptimal Behavior.Annie Gagliardi, Naomi H. Feldman & Jeffrey Lidz - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (1):188-217.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The Developmental Origins of Syntactic Bootstrapping.Cynthia Fisher, Kyong-sun Jin & Rose M. Scott - 2020 - Topics in Cognitive Science 12 (1):48-77.
    Fisher, Jin, and Scott push a central assumption of syntactic bootstrapping: that learners have a universal bias to map each noun in a sentence onto a participant role (i.e., argument of the verb). They propose two enrichments: First, that children use both semantic and syntactic information in representing nouns that accompany a verb; second, that children expect continuity across a discourse. They provide evidence for both learning mechanisms among young children, further spelling out the precise mechanisms underlying syntactic bootstrapping.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations