- Strategies for the control of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom.Gerald L. Gottlieb, Daniel M. Corcos & Gyan C. Agarwal - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):189-210.details
|
|
Adaptability of innate motor patterns and motor control mechanisms.M. B. Berkinblit, A. G. Feldman & O. I. Fukson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):585-599.details
|
|
Is human cognition adaptive?John R. Anderson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):471-485.details
|
|
The origin and use of positional frames of reference in motor control.Anatol G. Feldman & Mindy F. Levin - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):723-744.details
|
|
Strategies for the control of studies of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom.Gerale E. Loeb - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):227-227.details
|
|
Different regions of space or different spaces altogether: What are the dorsal/ventral systems processing?Gary W. Strong - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):556-557.details
|
|
If human cognition is adaptive, can human knowledge consist of encodings?Robert L. Campbell & Mark H. Bickhard - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):488-489.details
|
|
What are the building blocks of the frog's wiping reflex?Ilan Golani - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):607-608.details
|
|
Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in humans: Its ecological origins and neurophysiological implications.Fred H. Previc - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):519-542.details
|
|
Planning reaches by evaluating stored postures.David A. Rosenbaum, Loukia D. Loukopoulos, Ruud G. J. Meulenbroek, Jonathan Vaughan & Sascha E. Engelbrecht - 1995 - Psychological Review 102 (1):28-67.details
|
|
The prerequisites for one-jint motor control theories.S. V. Adamovich & A. G. Feldman - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):210-211.details
|
|
Simple changes in reflex threshold cannot explain all aspects of rapid voluntary movements.C. Gielen & J. C. Houk - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):605-607.details
|
|
Direct pattern-imposing control or dynamic regulation?Marl L. Latash - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):226-227.details
|
|
Braking may be more critical than acceleration.William A. MacKay - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):227-228.details
|
|
Visual processing in three-dimensional space: Perceptions and misperceptions.Fred H. Previc - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):559-575.details
|
|
Computational resources do constrain behavior.John K. Tsotsos - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):506-507.details
|
|
(1 other version)Speed, Accuracy, and Serial Order in Sequence Production.Peter Q. Pfordresher, Caroline Palmer & Melissa K. Jungers - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (1):63-98.details
|
|
In search of the theoretical basis of motor control.M. B. Berkinblit, A. G. Feldman & O. I. Fukson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):626-638.details
|
|
Do subprograms for movement always seek equilibrium?Z. Hasan - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):609-610.details
|
|
Does visual-field specialization really have implications for coordinated visual-motor behavior?Richard A. Abrams - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):542-543.details
|
|
Complexity in control of movements.Gyan C. Agarwal & Gerald L. Gottlieb - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):599-600.details
|
|
Human cognition is an adaptive process.Gyan C. Agarwal - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):485-486.details
|
|
Do legs have surplus degrees of freedom?R. McN Alexander - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):600-600.details
|
|
Is the multi-joint pointing movement model applicable to equilibrium control during upper trunk movements?Alexey Alexandrov, Alexander Frolov & Jean Massion - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-746.details
|
|
Tendon elasticity and positional control.R. McN Alexander - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-745.details
|
|
More on rational analysis.John R. Anderson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):508-517.details
|
|
Some thinking is irrational.Jonathan Baron - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):486-487.details
|
|
The nonoptimality of Anderson's memory fits.Gordon M. Becker - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):487-488.details
|
|
A few reasons why psychologlsts can adhere to Feldman and Levin's model.Mireille Bonnard & Jean Pailhous - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):746-747.details
|
|
Seeing double: Dichotomizing the visual system.R. Martyn Bracewell - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):543-544.details
|
|
The benefits and constraints of visual processing dichotomies.Julie R. Brannan - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):544-545.details
|
|
Ups and downs of the visual field: Manipulation and locomotion.Bruno G. Breitmeyer - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):545-546.details
|
|
Skeletal and oculomotor control systems compared.Bruce Bridgeman - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):212-212.details
|
|
Response field biases in parietal, temporal, and frontal lobe visual areas.Charles J. Bruce & Martha G. MacAvoy - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):546-547.details
|
|
Twisting the world by 90°.M. P. Bryden & Geoffrey Underwood - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):547-548.details
|
|
Saturation is not an evolutlonarily stable strategy.Daniel Bullock - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):212-214.details
|
|
Strategies and motor programs.Bruce D. Burns & Jeffery J. Summers - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):214-214.details
|
|
Functional specialization in the visual system: Retinotopic or body centered?Charles M. Butter - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):548-549.details
|
|
Variations of reflex parameters and their implications for the control of movements.Charles Capaday & Richard B. Stein - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):600-602.details
|
|
Mechanistic and rationalistic explanations are complementary.B. Chandrasekaran - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):489-491.details
|
|
The diversity of variability.William D. Chapple - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):602-602.details
|
|
Visual information in the upper and lower visual fields may be processed differently, but how and why remains to be established.Leo M. Chalupa & Cheryl A. White - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):549-550.details
|
|
Conservative or nonconservative control schemes.Daniel M. Corcos & Kerstin Pfann - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):747-749.details
|
|
Does constraining movements constrain the developement of movement theories?Daniel M. Corcos, Gerland L. Gottlieb & Gyan C. Agarwal - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):237-250.details
|
|
Movement strategies and the necessity for task differentiation.Daniel M. Corcos, Simon R. Gutman, Gyan C. Agarwal & Gerald L. Gottlieb - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):359-364.details
|
|
Normative theories of categorization.James E. Corter - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):491-492.details
|
|
On to real-life movements.Paul J. Cordo, Fay B. Horak & Susan P. Moore - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):214-215.details
|
|
The ups and downs of visual fields.David P. Crewther - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):550-551.details
|
|
Potential difficulties in the evaluation of motor strategies using EMG patterns.Warren G. Darling - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):352-353.details
|
|
The lambda model is only one piece in the motor control puzzle.Jeffrey Dean - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):749-749.details
|
|