- The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery.Marc Jeannerod - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):187-202.details
|
|
Is human cognition adaptive?John R. Anderson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):471-485.details
|
|
The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex.J. F. Stein - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):691-700.details
|
|
For effective sensorimotor processing must there be explicit representations and reconciliation of differing frames of reference?Garrett E. Alexander - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):321-322.details
|
|
Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements?S. C. Gandevia & David Burke - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):614-632.details
|
|
Coordinate transformation and limb movements: There may be more complexity than meets the eye.James R. Bloedel - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):326-326.details
|
|
Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation.Martha Flanders, Stephen I. Helms Tillery & John F. Soechting - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):309-320.details
|
|
Does the nervous system use equilibrium-point control to guide single and multiple joint movements?E. Bizzi, N. Hogan, F. A. Mussa-Ivaldi & S. Giszter - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):603-613.details
|
|
The origin and use of positional frames of reference in motor control.Anatol G. Feldman & Mindy F. Levin - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):723-744.details
|
|
Kinesthesia and unique solutions for control of multijoint movements.S. C. Gandevia - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):335-335.details
|
|
How does the nervous system control the equilibrium trajectory?S. V. Adamovich - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):704-705.details
|
|
Are errors in final position destined before the movement begins?Z. Hasan - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):341-342.details
|
|
Is stiffness the mainspring of posture and movement?Z. Hasan - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):756-758.details
|
|
Equilibrium-point hypothesis, minimum effort control strategy and the triphasic muscle activation pattern.Ning Lan & Patrick E. Crago - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):769-771.details
|
|
Coordinate transformations or dynamic models?Peter D. Neilson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):348-348.details
|
|
Jeannerod's representing brain: Image or illusion?Jean Pailhous & Mireille Bonnard - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):215-216.details
|
|
If human cognition is adaptive, can human knowledge consist of encodings?Robert L. Campbell & Mark H. Bickhard - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):488-489.details
|
|
Implications of neural networks for how we think about brain function.David A. Robinson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):644-655.details
|
|
Nonconscious motor images.Giacomo Rizzolatti - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):220-220.details
|
|
Call it what it is: Motor memory.Joaquin M. Fuster - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):208-208.details
|
|
Motor representations and reality.M. Jeannerod - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):229-245.details
|
|
Action and attention.A. H. C. Van der Heijden & Bruce Bridgeman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):225-226.details
|
|
On the relation between motor imagery and visual imagery.Roberta L. Klatzky - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):212-213.details
|
|
Cognitive and motor implications of mental imagery.Romeo Chua & Daniel J. Weeks - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):203-204.details
|
|
Moving beyond imagination.Robert Dufour, Martin H. Fischer & David A. Rosenbaum - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):206-207.details
|
|
Visual-spatial movement goals.Digby Elliott & Brian K. V. Maraj - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):207-207.details
|
|
Motor memory – a memory of the future.David H. Ingvar - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):210-211.details
|
|
Are motor images based on kinestheticvisual matching?Robert W. Mitchell - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):214-215.details
|
|
Motor images are action plans.Wolfgang Prinz - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):218-218.details
|
|
Separability of reference frame distinctions from motor and visual images.Gary W. Strong - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):224-225.details
|
|
Computational resources do constrain behavior.John K. Tsotsos - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):506-507.details
|
|
Physical modeling applies to physiology, too.Vincent Hayward - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):342-343.details
|
|
Motor simulation.Adam Morton - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):215-215.details
|
|
Involvement of primary motor cortex in motor imagery and mental practice.Mark Hallett, Jordan Fieldman, Leonardo G. Cohen, Norihiro Sadato & Alvaro Pascual-Leone - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):210-210.details
|
|
What are “normal movements” in atypical populations?Mark L. Latash & J. Greg Anson - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):55-68.details
|
|
The cerebellum and memory.Richard F. Thompson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):801-802.details
|
|
Human cognition is an adaptive process.Gyan C. Agarwal - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):485-486.details
|
|
Is the multi-joint pointing movement model applicable to equilibrium control during upper trunk movements?Alexey Alexandrov, Alexander Frolov & Jean Massion - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-746.details
|
|
Tendon elasticity and positional control.R. McN Alexander - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-745.details
|
|
More on rational analysis.John R. Anderson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):508-517.details
|
|
Toward peaceful coexistence of adaptive central strategies and medical professionals.J. Greg Anson & Mark L. Latash - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):94-106.details
|
|
Schemas, grasping, tensors and avoidance.Michael A. Arbib - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):322-323.details
|
|
Adaptive changes in postural reactions after unilateral leg amputation.Alexander S. Aruin - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):68-69.details
|
|
Do object affordances represent the functionality of an object?Ruzena Bajcsy - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):202-202.details
|
|
Some thinking is irrational.Jonathan Baron - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):486-487.details
|
|
The nonoptimality of Anderson's memory fits.Gordon M. Becker - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):487-488.details
|
|
Apparent approximations in sensorimotor transformations are due to errors in pointing.David J. Bennett & Eric P. Loeb - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):323-324.details
|
|
Bradykinesia in Parkinson's disease and cocontraction activity in dystonia are unlikely to be due to adaptive changes in the CNS.A. Berardelli, R. Agostino, A. Currà & M. Manfredi - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):69-69.details
|
|
Two paradoxes of pointing.Michail Berkinblit, Olga Fookson, Sergey Adamovich & Howard Poizner - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):324-325.details
|
|
Evaluation of central commands: Toward a theoretical basis for rehabilitation.Elena V. Biryukova, Alexandrez A. Frolov, Yves Burnod & Agnès Roby-Brami - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):69-71.details
|
|