Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. (1 other version)Phenomenological Claims and the Myth of the Given.Jean-Michel Roy - 2003 - Canadian Journal of Philosophy 33 (sup1):1-32.
    Over the past twenty years, Husserlian phenomenology has increasingly drawn the attention of the cognitive community, thereby leading to the emergence of what might be called a phenomenological trend within contemporary cognitive studies. What this phenomenological trend really amounts to is however a matter of debate. The reason is that it embodies, in fact, a multifaceted reflection about the relevance of Husserlian phenomenology to the current efforts towards a scientific theory of cognition, and, to a lesser degree, about the reciprocal (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Exploiting Multiple Sources of Information in Learning an Artificial Language: Human Data and Modeling.Pierre Perruchet & Barbara Tillmann - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (2):255-285.
    This study investigates the joint influences of three factors on the discovery of new word‐like units in a continuous artificial speech stream: the statistical structure of the ongoing input, the initial word‐likeness of parts of the speech flow, and the contextual information provided by the earlier emergence of other word‐like units. Results of an experiment conducted with adult participants show that these sources of information have strong and interactive influences on word discovery. The authors then examine the ability of different (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Mental Representation and Closely Conflated Topics.Angela Mendelovici - 2010 - Dissertation, Princeton University
    This dissertation argues that mental representation is identical to phenomenal consciousness, and everything else that appears to be both mental and a matter of representation is not genuine mental representation, but either in some way derived from mental representation, or a case of non-mental representation.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • Concrete magnitudes: From numbers to time.Christine Falter, Valdas Noreika, Julian Kiverstein & Bruno Mölder - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3-4):335-336.
    Cohen Kadosh & Walsh (CK&W) present convincing evidence indicating the existence of notation-specific numerical representations in parietal cortex. We suggest that the same conclusions can be drawn for a particular type of numerical representation: the representation of time. Notation-dependent representations need not be limited to number but may also be extended to other magnitude-related contents processed in parietal cortex (Walsh 2003).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Describing one’s subjective experience in the second person: An interview method for the science of consciousness. [REVIEW]Claire Petitmengin - 2006 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 5 (3-4):229-269.
    This article presents an interview method which enables us to bring a person, who may not even have been trained, to become aware of his or her subjective experience, and describe it with great precision. It is focused on the difficulties of becoming aware of one’s subjective experience and describing it, and on the processes used by this interview technique to overcome each of these difficulties. The article ends with a discussion of the criteria governing the validity of the descriptions (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   130 citations  
  • First- and third-person approaches in implicit learning research.Vinciane Gaillard, Muriel Vandenberghe, Arnaud Destrebecqz & Axel Cleeremans - 2006 - Consciousness and Cognition 15 (4):709-722.
    How do we find out whether someone is conscious of some information or not? A simple answer is “We just ask them”! However, things are not so simple. Here, we review recent developments in the use of subjective and objective methods in implicit learning research and discuss the highly complex methodological problems that their use raises in the domain.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • A Recurrent Connectionist Model of Melody Perception: An Exploration Using TRACX2.Daniel Defays, Robert M. French & Barbara Tillmann - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (4):e13283.
    Are similar, or even identical, mechanisms used in the computational modeling of speech segmentation, serial image processing, and music processing? We address this question by exploring how TRACX2, a recognition‐based, recursive connectionist autoencoder model of chunking and sequence segmentation, which has successfully simulated speech and serial‐image processing, might be applied to elementary melody perception. The model, a three‐layer autoencoder that recognizes “chunks” of short sequences of intervals that have been frequently encountered on input, is trained on the tone intervals of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Emergence of Explicit Knowledge in a Serial Reaction Time Task: The Role of Experienced Fluency and Strength of Representation.Sarah Esser & Hilde Haider - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Regularity Extraction Across Species: Associative Learning Mechanisms Shared by Human and Non‐Human Primates.Arnaud Rey, Laure Minier, Raphaëlle Malassis, Louisa Bogaerts & Joël Fagot - 2019 - Topics in Cognitive Science 11 (3):573-586.
    One of the themes that has been widely addressed in both the implicit learning and statistical learning literatures is that of rule learning. While it is widely agreed that the extraction of regularities from the environment is a fundamental facet of cognition, there is still debate about the nature of rule learning. Rey and colleagues show that the comparison between human and non‐human primates can contribute important insights to this debate.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Musical structure modulates semantic priming in vocal music.Bénédicte Poulin-Charronnat, Emmanuel Bigand, François Madurell & Ronald Peereman - 2005 - Cognition 94 (3):B67-B78.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Two ways of learning associations.Luke Boucher & Zoltán Dienes - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (6):807-842.
    How people learn chunks or associations between adjacent items in sequences was modelled. Two previously successful models of how people learn artificial grammars were contrasted: the CCN, a network version of the competitive chunker of Servan‐Schreiber and Anderson [J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 16 (1990) 592], which produces local and compositionally‐structured chunk representations acquired incrementally; and the simple recurrent network (SRN) of Elman [Cogn. Sci. 14 (1990) 179], which acquires distributed representations through error correction. The models' susceptibility to two (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • In search of non-abstract representation of numbers: Maybe on the right track, but still not there.Joseph Tzelgov & Michal Pinhas - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3-4):353 - 354.
    We agree that the default numerical representation is best accessed by probing automatic processing. The locus of this representation is apparently at the horizontal intraparietal sulcus (HIPS), the convergence zone of magnitude information. The parietal lobes are the right place to look for non-abstract representation of magnitude, yet the proof for that is still to be found.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Language learning in infancy: Does the empirical evidence support a domain specific language acquisition device?Christina Behme & Helene Deacon - 2008 - Philosophical Psychology 21 (5):641 – 671.
    Poverty of the Stimulus Arguments have convinced many linguists and philosophers of language that a domain specific language acquisition device (LAD) is necessary to account for language learning. Here we review empirical evidence that casts doubt on the necessity of this domain specific device. We suggest that more attention needs to be paid to the early stages of language acquisition. Many seemingly innate language-related abilities have to be learned over the course of several months. Further, the language input contains rich (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Two types of thought: Evidence from aphasia.Jules Davidoff - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (1):20-21.
    Evidence from aphasia is considered that leads to a distinction between abstract and concrete thought processes and hence for a distinction between rules and similarity. It is argued that perceptual classification is inherently a rule-following procedure and these rules are unable to be followed when a patient has difficulty with name comprehension and retrieval.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: A critical review of visual masking.Sid Kouider & Stanislas Dehaene - 2007 - Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B 362 (1481):857-875.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   97 citations  
  • What is a Computer? A Survey.William J. Rapaport - 2018 - Minds and Machines 28 (3):385-426.
    A critical survey of some attempts to define ‘computer’, beginning with some informal ones, then critically evaluating those of three philosophers, and concluding with an examination of whether the brain and the universe are computers.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Can the exploration of left space be induced implicitly in unilateral neglect?Murielle Wansard, Paolo Bartolomeo, Valérie Vanderaspoilden, Marie Geurten & Thierry Meulemans - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 31:115-123.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • (1 other version)Implicit learning and statistical learning: One phenomenon, two approaches.Pierre Perruchet & Sebastien Pacton - 2006 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (5):233-238.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   81 citations  
  • The phenomenology of endogenous orienting.Paolo Bartolomeo, Caroline Decaix & Eric Siéroff - 2007 - Consciousness and Cognition 16 (1):144-161.
    Can we build endogenous expectations about the locus of occurrence of a target without being able to describe them? Participants performed cue–target detection tasks with different proportions of valid and invalid trials, without being informed of these proportions, and demonstrated typical endogenous effects. About half were subsequently able to correctly describe the cue–target relationships . However, even non-verbalizer participants showed endogenous orienting with peripheral cues , not depending solely on practice . Explicit instructions did not bring about dramatic advantages in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The Evolution of Chunks in Sequence Learning.Laure Tosatto, Joël Fagot, Dezso Nemeth & Arnaud Rey - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (4).
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 4, April 2022.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • On the automaticity of pure perceptual sequence learning.Daphné Coomans, Natacha Deroost, Peter Zeischka & Eric Soetens - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4):1460-1472.
    We investigated the automaticity of implicit sequence learning by varying perceptual load in a pure perceptual sequence learning paradigm. Participants responded to the randomly changing identity of a target, while the irrelevant target location was structured. In Experiment 1, the target was presented under low or high perceptual load during training, whereas testing occurred without load. Unexpectedly, no sequence learning was observed. In Experiment 2, perceptual load was introduced during the test phase to determine whether load is required to express (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The rules versus similarity distinction.Emmanuel M. Pothos - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (1):1-14.
    The distinction between rules and similarity is central to our understanding of much of cognitive psychology. Two aspects of existing research have motivated the present work. First, in different cognitive psychology areas we typically see different conceptions of rules and similarity; for example, rules in language appear to be of a different kind compared to rules in categorization. Second, rules processes are typically modeled as separate from similarity ones; for example, in a learning experiment, rules and similarity influences would be (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • A Neurocognitive Framework for Human Creative Thought.Arne Dietrich & Hilde Haider - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • What Mechanisms Underlie Implicit Statistical Learning? Transitional Probabilities Versus Chunks in Language Learning.Pierre Perruchet - 2019 - Topics in Cognitive Science 11 (3):520-535.
    In 2006, Perruchet and Pacton (2006) asked whether implicit learning and statistical learning represent two approaches to the same phenomenon. This article represents an important follow‐up to their seminal review article. As in the previous paper, the focus is on the formation of elementary cognitive units. Both approaches favor different explanations on what these units consist of and how they are formed. Perruchet weighs up the evidence for different explanations and concludes with a helpful agenda for future research.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Implicit learning: A way to improve visual search in spatial neglect?Murielle Wansard, Marie Geurten, Catherine Colson & Thierry Meulemans - 2016 - Consciousness and Cognition 43:102-112.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is Consciousness a Passive Recipient of the End-Product of Sophisticated Unconscious Computations?Pierre Perruchet & Bénédicte Poulin-Charronnat - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Relating magnitudes: the brain's code for proportions.Simon N. Jacob, Daniela Vallentin & Andreas Nieder - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (3):157-166.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • ADAPT: A Developmental, Asemantic, and Procedural Model for Transcoding From Verbal to Arabic Numerals.Pierre Barrouillet, Valérie Camos, Pierre Perruchet & Xavier Seron - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (2):368-394.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • The Temporal Dynamics of Regularity Extraction in Non‐Human Primates.Laure Minier, Joël Fagot & Arnaud Rey - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (4):1019-1030.
    Extracting the regularities of our environment is one of our core cognitive abilities. To study the fine-grained dynamics of the extraction of embedded regularities, a method combining the advantages of the artificial language paradigm and the serial response time task was used with a group of Guinea baboons in a new automatic experimental device. After a series of random trials, monkeys were exposed to language-like patterns. We found that the extraction of embedded patterns positioned at the end of larger patterns (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Subliminal speech perception and auditory streaming.Emmanuel Dupoux, Vincent de Gardelle & Sid Kouider - 2008 - Cognition 109 (2):267-273.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Can musical transformations be implicitly learned?Zoltan Dienes & Christopher Longuet-Higgins - 2004 - Cognitive Science 28 (4):531-558.
    The dominant theory of what people can learn implicitly is that they learn chunks of adjacent elements in sequences. A type of musical grammar that goes beyond specifying allowable chunks is provided by serialist or 12‐tone music. The rules constitute operations over variables and could not be appreciated as such by a system that can only chunk elements together. A series of studies investigated the extent to which people could implicitly (or explicitly) learn the structures of serialist music. We found (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Implicit sequence learning and conscious awareness.Qiufang Fu, Xiaolan Fu & Zoltán Dienes - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (1):185-202.
    This paper uses the Process Dissociation Procedure to explore whether people can acquire unconscious knowledge in the serial reaction time task [Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. . Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the Process Dissociation Procedure. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 343–350; Wilkinson, L., & Shanks, D. R. . Intentional control and implicit sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 354–369]. Experiment 1 showed that people generated legal sequences above baseline levels under exclusion (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • The interoception and imagination loop in hypnotic phenomena.Alain Parra & Arnaud Rey - 2019 - Consciousness and Cognition 73:102765.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Brain neural activity patterns yielding numbers are operators, not representations.Walter J. Freeman & Robert Kozma - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3-4):336.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Inattentional awareness.Donelson E. Dulany - 2001 - PSYCHE: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research On Consciousness 7.
    The authors report "priming" effects for subjects they classify as "inattentionally blind" and interpret this as evidence for unconscious perception--an interpretation consistent with deeply entrenched metatheory. I question that interpretation, however, on methodological grounds. On these assessment procedures, some subjects could be classified as "inattentionally blind" despite representing the critical stimulus in conscious attention. Still others--presenting a more interesting challenge--could be so classified despite representing the stimulus literally in inattentional awareness. The study is illuminated, I believe, by seeing it in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation