Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The Sound of Slurs: Bad Sounds for Bad Words.Eric Mandelbaum & Steven Young - forthcoming - Oxford Studies in Experimental Philosophy.
    An analysis of a valenced corpus of English words revealed that words that rhyme with slurs are rated more poorly than their synonyms. What at first might seem like a bizarre coincidence turns out to be a robust feature of slurs, one arising from their phonetic structure. We report novel data on phonaesthetic preferences, showing that a particular class of phonemes are both particularly disliked, and overrepresented in slurs. We argue that phonaesthetic associations have been an overlooked source of some (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Japanese Sound-Symbolic Words for Representing the Hardness of an Object Are Judged Similarly by Japanese and English Speakers.Li Shan Wong, Jinhwan Kwon, Zane Zheng, Suzy J. Styles, Maki Sakamoto & Ryo Kitada - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Contrary to the assumption of arbitrariness in modern linguistics, sound symbolism, which is the non-arbitrary relationship between sounds and meanings, exists. Sound symbolism, including the “Bouba–Kiki” effect, implies the universality of such relationships; individuals from different cultural and linguistic backgrounds can similarly relate sound-symbolic words to referents, although the extent of these similarities remains to be fully understood. Here, we examined if subjects from different countries could similarly infer the surface texture properties from words that sound-symbolically represent hardness in Japanese. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Automatic Estimation of Multidimensional Personality From a Single Sound-Symbolic Word.Maki Sakamoto, Junji Watanabe & Koichi Yamagata - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Researchers typically use the “big five” traits as a standard way to describe personality. Evaluation of personality is generally conducted using self-report questionnaires that require participants to respond to a large number of test items. To minimize the burden on participants, this paper proposes an alternative method of estimating multidimensional personality traits from only a single word. We constructed a system that can convert a sound-symbolic word that intuitively expresses personality traits into information expressed by 50 personality-related adjective pairs. This (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Boundaries in space and time: Iconic biases across modalities.Jeremy Kuhn, Carlo Geraci, Philippe Schlenker & Brent Strickland - 2021 - Cognition 210 (C):104596.
    The idea that the form of a word reflects information about its meaning has its roots in Platonic philosophy, and has been experimentally investigated for concrete, sensory-based properties since the early 20th century. Here, we provide evidence for an abstract property of ‘boundedness’ that introduces a systematic, iconic bias on the phonological expectations of a novel lexicon. We show that this abstract property is general across events and objects. In Experiment 1, we show that subjects are systematically more likely to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Stimulus Parameters Underlying Sound‐Symbolic Mapping of Auditory Pseudowords to Visual Shapes.Simon Lacey, Yaseen Jamal, Sara M. List, K. Sathian & Lynne C. Nygaard - 2020 - Cognitive Science 44 (9):e12883.
    Sound symbolism refers to non‐arbitrary mappings between the sounds of words and their meanings and is often studied by pairing auditory pseudowords such as “maluma” and “takete” with rounded and pointed visual shapes, respectively. However, it is unclear what auditory properties of pseudowords contribute to their perception as rounded or pointed. Here, we compared perceptual ratings of the roundedness/pointedness of large sets of pseudowords and shapes to their acoustic and visual properties using a novel application of representational similarity analysis (RSA). (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Korean Mothers Attune the Frequency and Acoustic Saliency of Sound Symbolic Words to the Linguistic Maturity of Their Children.Jinyoung Jo & Eon-Suk Ko - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Iconicity in the lab: a review of behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging research into sound-symbolism.Gwilym Lockwood & Mark Dingemanse - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:1-14.
    This review covers experimental approaches to sound-symbolism—from infants to adults, and from Sapir’s foundational studies to twenty-first century product naming. It synthesizes recent behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging work into a systematic overview of the cross-modal correspondences that underpin iconic links between form and meaning. It also identifies open questions and opportunities, showing how the future course of experimental iconicity research can benefit from an integrated interdisciplinary perspective. Combining insights from psychology and neuroscience with evidence from natural languages provides us with (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • What is the link between synaesthesia and sound symbolism?Kaitlyn Bankieris & Julia Simner - 2015 - Cognition 136 (C):186-195.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Effects of phonetic symbolism on paired-associate learning.Ronald A. Cohen & Chizuko Izawa - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 8 (6):475-478.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Inherent emotional quality of human speech sounds.Blake Myers-Schulz, Maia Pujara, Richard C. Wolf & Michael Koenigs - 2013 - Cognition and Emotion 27 (6):1105-1113.
    During much of the past century, it was widely believed that phonemes--the human speech sounds that constitute words--have no inherent semantic meaning, and that the relationship between a combination of phonemes (a word) and its referent is simply arbitrary. Although recent work has challenged this picture by revealing psychological associations between certain phonemes and particular semantic contents, the precise mechanisms underlying these associations have not been fully elucidated. Here we provide novel evidence that certain phonemes have an inherent, non-arbitrary emotional (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Johann Nikolaus Tetens (1736–1807) and the Idea of Phoneme: A Chapter in the History of Linguistic Thought.Pierluigi D’Agostino - 2024 - Hopos: The Journal of the International Society for the History of Philosophy of Science 14 (1):185-209.
    In this article, I focus on Johann Nikolaus Tetens’s linguistic theory to make three arguments: (a) this linguistic theory endorses a phonological (contra phonetic) approach to the acoustic sphere of language; (b) the phonological approach is based on the idea that sounds can turn into phonemes (of a properly human language) only when a minimally rational reflection on them is made; and (c) the phonological approach allows us to understand the phoneme as a differential unity, as being composed of structure (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cross-modal iconicity and indexicality in the production of lexical sensory and emotional signs in Finnish Sign Language.Jarkko Keränen - 2023 - Cognitive Linguistics 34 (3-4):333-369.
    In the present study, cross-modal (i.e., across sensory modalities such as smell and sound) iconicity (i.e., resemblance) and indexicality (i.e., contiguity) in lexical sensory and emotional signs in Finnish Sign Language will be considered from an articulatory perspective (i.e., the production of signs). Such cross-modal iconicity has not been extensively studied previously, so here, with the help of cognitive semiotics, I aim to carefully describe the cross-modal patterns observed across 118 signs, including 60 sensory signs and 58 emotional signs. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Iconicity and systematicity in phonaesthemes: A cross-linguistic study.Javier Valenzuela, Amandine Fregier & Jose A. Mompean - 2020 - Cognitive Linguistics 31 (3):515-548.
    This study aims to find out whether speakers of different language backgrounds (English, French, Spanish, and Macedonian) are sensitive to semantic associations (‘fluid’ and ‘forcible contact’) attached respectively to two purported phonaesthemes (/fl-/ and /tr-/). Participants completed the task in oral and written conditions. They had to match phonaestheme-related definitions with either of two non-words (one phonaestheme-bearing and the other containing a distractor). The results obtained indicate that participants significantly chose non-words beginning with /tr-/ when the definition activated a meaning (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Debunking two myths against vocal origins of language.Marcus Perlman - 2017 - Interaction Studies 18 (3):376-401.
    Gesture-first theories of language origins often raise two unsubstantiated arguments against vocal origins. First, they argue that great ape vocal behavior is highly constrained, limited to a fixed, species-typical repertoire of reflexive calls. Second, they argue that vocalizations lack any significant potential to ground meaning through iconicity, or resemblance between form and meaning. This paper reviews the considerable evidence that debunks these two “myths”. Accumulating evidence shows that the great apes exercise voluntary control over their vocal behavior, including their breathing (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Words cluster phonetically beyond phonotactic regularities.Isabelle Dautriche, Kyle Mahowald, Edward Gibson, Anne Christophe & Steven T. Piantadosi - 2017 - Cognition 163 (C):128-145.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • The sound of distance.Cristina D. Rabaglia, Sam J. Maglio, Madelaine Krehm, Jin H. Seok & Yaacov Trope - 2016 - Cognition 152:141-149.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Japanese Sound-Symbolism Facilitates Word Learning in English-Speaking Children.Katerina Kantartzis, Mutsumi Imai & Sotaro Kita - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (3):575-586.
    Sound-symbolism is the nonarbitrary link between the sound and meaning of a word. Japanese-speaking children performed better in a verb generalization task when they were taught novel sound-symbolic verbs, created based on existing Japanese sound-symbolic words, than novel nonsound-symbolic verbs (Imai, Kita, Nagumo, & Okada, 2008). A question remained as to whether the Japanese children had picked up regularities in the Japanese sound-symbolic lexicon or were sensitive to universal sound-symbolism. The present study aimed to provide support for the latter. In (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Sounds exaggerate visual shape.Timothy D. Sweeny, Emmanuel Guzman-Martinez, Laura Ortega, Marcia Grabowecky & Satoru Suzuki - 2012 - Cognition 124 (2):194-200.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The emergence of word order from a social network perspective.Shiri Lev-Ari - 2023 - Cognition 237 (C):105466.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Implicit Association Test (IAT) Studies Investigating Pitch‐Shape Audiovisual Cross‐modal Associations Across Language Groups.Nan Shang & Suzy J. Styles - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (1):e13221.
    Previous studies have shown that Chinese speakers and non-Chinese speakers exhibit different patterns of cross-modal congruence for the lexical tones of Mandarin Chinese, depending on which features of the pitch they attend to. But is this pattern of language-specific listening a conscious cultural strategy or an automatic processing effect? If automatic, does it also apply when the same pitch contours no longer sound like speech? Implicit Association Tests (IATs) provide an indirect measure of cross-modal association. In a series of IAT (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “Big” Sounds Bigger in More Widely Spoken Languages.Shiri Lev-Ari, Ivet Kancheva, Louise Marston, Hannah Morris, Teah Swingler & Madina Zaynudinova - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (11):e13059.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 45, Issue 11, November 2021.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Phonation Types Matter in Sound Symbolism.Kimi Akita - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (5):e12982.
    Sound symbolism is a non‐arbitrary correspondence between sound and meaning. The majority of studies on sound symbolism have focused on consonants and vowels, and the sound‐symbolic properties of suprasegmentals, particularly phonation types, have been largely neglected. This study examines the size and shape symbolism of four phonation types: modal and creaky voices, falsetto, and whisper. Japanese speakers heard 12 novel words (e.g., /íbi/, /ápa/) pronounced with the four types of phonation and rated the size and roundedness/pointedness each of the 48 (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What's in and what's out in branding? A novel articulation effect for brand names.Sascha Topolinski, Michael Zürn & Iris K. Schneider - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Non-lexical conversational sounds in American English.Nigel Ward - 2006 - Pragmatics and Cognition 14 (1):129-182.
    Sounds like h-nmm, hh-aaaah, hn-hn, unkay, nyeah, ummum, uuh, um-hm-uh-hm, um and uh-huh occur frequently in American English conversation but have thus far escaped systematic study. This article reports a study of both the forms and functions of such tokens in a corpus of American English conversations. These sounds appear not to be lexical, in that they are productively generated rather than finite in number, and in that the sound¿meaning mapping is compositional rather than arbitrary. This implies that English bears (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Sound symbolic associations in Spanish emotional words: affective dimensions and discrete emotions.Rocío Calvillo-Torres, Juan Haro, Pilar Ferré, Claudia Poch & José A. Hinojosa - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    Sound symbolism refers to non-arbitrary associations between word forms and meaning, such as those observed for some properties of sounds and size or shape. Recent evidence suggests that these connections extend to emotional concepts. Here we investigated two types of non-arbitrary relationships. Study 1 examined whether iconicity scores (i.e. resemblance-based mapping between aspects of a word’s form and its meaning) for words can be predicted from ratings in the affective dimensions of valence and arousal and/or the discrete emotions of happiness, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Guessing Meaning From Word Sounds of Unfamiliar Languages: A Cross-Cultural Sound Symbolism Study.Anita D’Anselmo, Giulia Prete, Przemysław Zdybek, Luca Tommasi & Alfredo Brancucci - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The poetry of sound and the sound of poetry: Navajo poetry, phonological iconicity, and linguistic relativity.Anthony K. Webster - 2015 - Semiotica 2015 (207):279-301.
    Name der Zeitschrift: Semiotica Jahrgang: 2015 Heft: 207 Seiten: 279-301.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Iconic Prosody in Story Reading.Marcus Perlman, Nathaniel Clark & Marlene Johansson Falck - 2015 - Cognitive Science 39 (6):1348-1368.
    Recent experiments have shown that people iconically modulate their prosody corresponding with the meaning of their utterance. This article reports findings from a story reading task that expands the investigation of iconic prosody to abstract meanings in addition to concrete ones. Participants read stories that contrasted along concrete and abstract semantic dimensions of speed and size. Participants read fast stories at a faster rate than slow stories, and big stories with a lower pitch than small stories. The effect of speed (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • What is preferred in the in–out effect: articulation locations or articulation movement direction?Anita Körner & Ralf Rummer - 2022 - Cognition and Emotion 36 (2):230-239.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Sound symbolism in Chinese children’s literature.Xiaoxi Wang - 2022 - Cognitive Linguistics 33 (1):95-120.
    Iconicity is a fundamental property of spoken and signed languages. However, quantitative analysis of sound-meaning association in Chinese has not been extensively developed, and little is known about the impact of sound symbolism in children’s literature. As sound symbolism is supposed to be a universal cognitive phenomenon, this research seeks to investigate whether iconic structures of Mandarin are embodied in native Chinese speakers’ language experience. The paper describes a case study of Chinese storybooks with the goal of testing whether phonosemantic (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Umwelt and Ape Language Experiments: on the Role of Iconicity in the Human-Ape Pidgin Language.Mirko Cerrone - 2018 - Biosemiotics 11 (1):41-63.
    Several language experiments have been carried out on apes and other animals aiming to narrow down the presumed qualitative gap that separates humans from other animals. These experiments, however, have been driven by the understanding of language as a purely symbolic sign system, often connected to a profound disinterest for language use in real situations and a propensity to perceive grammatical and syntactic information as the only fundamental aspects of human language. For these reasons, the language taught to apes tends (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Which words are most iconic?Bodo Winter, Marcus Perlman, Lynn K. Perry & Gary Lupyan - 2017 - Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies / Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies 18 (3):443-464.
    Some spoken words are iconic, exhibiting a resemblance between form and meaning. We used native speaker ratings to assess the iconicity of 3001 English words, analyzing their iconicity in relation to part-of-speech differences and differences between the sensory domain they relate to. First, we replicated previous findings showing that onomatopoeia and interjections were highest in iconicity, followed by verbs and adjectives, and then nouns and grammatical words. We further show that words with meanings related to the senses are more iconic (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • The Whale and the Microorganism: A Tale of a Classic Example and Linguistic Intuitions.Shiri Lev-Ari - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (4):e13287.
    A classic example of the arbitrary relation between the way a word sounds and its meaning is that microorganism is a very long word that refers to a very small entity, whereas whale is a very short word that refers to something very big. This example, originally presented in Hockett's list of language's design features, has been often cited over the years, not only by those discussing the arbitrary nature of language, but also by researchers of sound symbolism. While the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A Cross‐Modal and Cross‐lingual Study of Iconicity in Language: Insights From Deep Learning.Andrea Gregor de Varda & Carlo Strapparava - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (6):e13147.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 6, June 2022.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Structural Effects of Modality on the Rise of Symbolic Language: A Rebuttal of Evolutionary Accounts and a Laboratory Demonstration.Victor J. Boucher, Annie C. Gilbert & Antonin Rossier-Bisaillon - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:305809.
    Why does symbolic communication in humans develop primarily in an oral medium, and how do theories of language origin explain this? Non-human primates, despite their ability to learn and use symbolic signs, do not develop symbols as in oral language. This partly owes to the lack of a direct cortico-motoneuron control of vocalizations in these species compared to humans. Yet such modality-related factors that can impinge on the rise of symbolic language are interpreted differently in two types of evolutionary storylines. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Bouba/Kiki in Touch: Associations Between Tactile Perceptual Qualities and Japanese Phonemes.Maki Sakamoto & Junji Watanabe - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Visual and Proprioceptive Perceptions Evoke Motion-Sound Symbolism: Different Acceleration Profiles Are Associated With Different Types of Consonants.Kazuko Shinohara, Shigeto Kawahara & Hideyuki Tanaka - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sound Predicts Meaning: Cross‐Modal Associations Between Formant Frequency and Emotional Tone in Stanzas.Jan Auracher, Winfried Menninghaus & Mathias Scharinger - 2020 - Cognitive Science 44 (10):e12906.
    Research on the relation between sound and meaning in language has reported substantial evidence for implicit associations between articulatory–acoustic characteristics of phonemes and emotions. In the present study, we specifically tested the relation between the acoustic properties of a text and its emotional tone as perceived by readers. To this end, we asked participants to assess the emotional tone of single stanzas extracted from a large variety of poems. The selected stanzas had either an extremely high, a neutral, or an (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The Sound of Grasp Affordances: Influence of Grasp‐Related Size of Categorized Objects on Vocalization.Lari Vainio, Martti Vainio, Jari Lipsanen & Rob Ellis - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (10):e12793.
    Previous research shows that simultaneously executed grasp and vocalization responses are faster when the precision grip is performed with the vowel [i] and the power grip is performed with the vowel [ɑ]. Research also shows that observing an object that is graspable with a precision or power grip can activate the grip congruent with the object. Given the connection between vowel articulation and grasping, this study explores whether grasp‐related size of observed objects can influence not only grasp responses but also (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is a High Tone Pointy? Speakers of Different Languages Match Mandarin Chinese Tones to Visual Shapes Differently.Nan Shang & Suzy J. Styles - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • What Does a Horgous Look Like? Nonsense Words Elicit Meaningful Drawings.Charles P. Davis, Hannah M. Morrow & Gary Lupyan - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (10):e12791.
    To what extent do people attribute meanings to “nonsense” words? How general is such attribution of meaning? We used a set of words lacking conventional meanings to elicit drawings of made‐up creatures. Separate groups of participants rated the nonsense words and the drawings on several semantic dimensions and selected what name best corresponded to each creature. Despite lacking conventional meanings, “nonsense” words elicited a high level of consistency in the produced drawings. Meaning attributions made to nonsense words corresponded with meaning (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Sensus Communis: Some Perspectives on the Origins of Non-synchronous Cross-Sensory Associations.Bahia Guellaï, Annabel Callin, Frédéric Bevilacqua, Diemo Schwarz, Alexandre Pitti, Sofiane Boucenna & Maya Gratier - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • ERPs reveal an iconic relation between sublexical phonology and affective meaning.M. Conrad, S. Ullrich, D. Schmidtke & S. A. Kotz - 2022 - Cognition 226 (C):105182.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Phonological Iconicity Electrifies: An ERP Study on Affective Sound-to-Meaning Correspondences in German.Susann Ullrich, Sonja A. Kotz, David S. Schmidtke, Arash Aryani & Markus Conrad - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Form and Function of Poetic Language.Ivan Fónagy - 1965 - Diogenes 13 (51):72-110.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cross-modal iconicity.Felix Ahlner & Jordan Zlatev - 2010 - Sign Systems Studies 38 (1-4):298-346.
    It is being increasingly recognized that the Saussurean dictum of “the arbitrariness of the linguistic sign” is in conflict with the pervasiveness of the phenomenon commonly known as “sound symbolism”. After first presenting a historical overview of the debate, however, we conclude that both positions have been exaggerated, and that an adequate explanation of sound symbolism is still lacking. How can there, for example, be (perceived) similarity between expressionsand contents across different sensory modalities? We offer an answer, based on the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Sound symbolism facilitates early verb learning.Mutsumi Imai, Sotaro Kita, Miho Nagumo & Hiroyuki Okada - 2008 - Cognition 109 (1):54-65.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • Exploring Tactile Perceptual Dimensions Using Materials Associated with Sensory Vocabulary.Maki Sakamoto & Junji Watanabe - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • On the Relation between the General Affective Meaning and the Basic Sublexical, Lexical, and Inter-lexical Features of Poetic Texts—A Case Study Using 57 Poems of H. M. Enzensberger.Susann Ullrich, Arash Aryani, Maria Kraxenberger, Arthur M. Jacobs & Markus Conrad - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Emotional sound symbolism: Languages rapidly signal valence via phonemes.James S. Adelman, Zachary Estes & Martina Cossu - 2018 - Cognition 175 (C):122-130.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations