Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Passive frame theory: A new synthesis.Ezequiel Morsella, Christine A. Godwin, Tiffany K. Jantz, Stephen C. Krieger & Adam Gazzaley - 2016 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39:e199.
    Passive frame theory attempts to illuminate what consciousnessis, in mechanistic and functional terms; it does not address the “implementation” level of analysis (how neurons instantiate conscious states), an enigma for various disciplines. However, in response to the commentaries, we discuss how our framework provides clues regarding this enigma. In the framework, consciousness is passive albeit essential. Without consciousness, there would not be adaptive skeletomotor action.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Naive geography and geopolitical semiotics: The semiotic analysis of geomental maps of Russians.Natalya Zelyanskaya, Konstantin Belousov & Dilara Ichkineeva - 2017 - Semiotica 2017 (215):235-253.
    Name der Zeitschrift: Semiotica Jahrgang: 2017 Heft: 215 Seiten: 235-253.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dissociable functions of reward inference in the lateral prefrontal cortex and the striatum.Shingo Tanaka, Xiaochuan Pan, Mineki Oguchi, Jessica E. Taylor & Masamichi Sakagami - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Spatial navigation, episodic memory, episodic future thinking, and theory of mind in children with autism spectrum disorder: evidence for impairments in mental simulation?Sophie E. Lind, Dermot M. Bowler & Jacob Raber - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5:113592.
    This study explored spatial navigation alongside several other cognitive abilities that are thought to share common underlying neurocognitive mechanisms (e.g., the capacity for self-projection, scene construction, or mental simulation), and which we hypothesised may be impaired in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Twenty intellectually high-functioning children with ASD (with a mean age of ~8 years) were compared to 20 sex, age, IQ, and language ability matched typically developing children on a series of tasks to assess spatial navigation, episodic memory, episodic future (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Training of Tonal Similarity Ratings in Non-Musicians: A “Rapid Learning” Approach.Mathias S. Oechslin, Damian Läge & Oliver Vitouch - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task.S. Gouteux, C. Thinus-Blanc & J. Vauclair - 2001 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130 (3):505.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Mental Models, Psychology of.J. M. Loomis, R. L. Klatzky, R. G. Golledge & J. G. CicineIli - 1991 - In Stephen Everson (ed.), Psychology: Companions to Ancient Thought, Vol. 2. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 56-89.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Earth is flat when personally significant experiences with the sphericity of the Earth are absent.Claus-Christian Carbon - 2010 - Cognition 116 (1):130-135.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Psychology and epistemology: The place versus response controversy.Ron Amundson - 1985 - Cognition 20 (2):127-153.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Updating egocentric representations in human navigation.Ranxiao Frances Wang & Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2000 - Cognition 77 (3):215-250.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  • Hippocampus, memory and movement.Abram Amsel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):494-495.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Hippocampus and memory.Raymond P. Kesner - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):509-510.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The cognitive map as a hippocampus.John O'Keefe & Lynn Nadel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):520-533.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   62 citations  
  • Ecologizing world graphs.Robert E. Shaw & Ennio Mingolla - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):648-650.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Standards for neural modeling.Jerome A. Feldman & David Zipser - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):642-642.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The special nature of spatial information.Michael Potegal - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):647-648.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The heterogeneity and plasticity of cerebral structures.Bruno E. Will, John C. Dalrymple-Alford & Georges Di Scala - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):131-132.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Hippocampal–collicular interactions: An example of input linkages to the hippocampus.Thomas L. Bennett - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):119-119.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Behavioral evidence of thought and consciousness [G].D. O. Hebb - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (4):577-577.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Affordance perception and the Y-magnocellular pathway.Chris Fields - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):403-404.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mechanism at two thousand.John C. Marshall - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):637.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ontology and ideology of behaviorism and mentalism.Georges Rey - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):640.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The behaviorist concept of mind.David M. Rosenthal - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):643.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Implementations, algorithms, and more.John R. Anderson - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):498-505.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The response problem.Robert C. Bolles - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):135-136.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • What do reinforcers strengthen? The unit of selection.John W. Donahoe - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):138-139.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Connectionism and implementation.Paul Smolensky - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):492-493.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Connectionism and motivation are compatible.Daniel S. Levine - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):487-487.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Moving beyond schedules and rate: A new trajectory?Gregory Galbicka - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):139-140.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Integration and specificity of retrieval in a memory-based model of reinforcement.Marvin D. Krank - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):142-143.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From overt behavior to hypothetical behavior to memory: Inference in the wrong direction.Howard Rachlin - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):147-148.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   66 citations  
  • A unified framework for addiction: Vulnerabilities in the decision process.Adam Johnson A. David Redish, Steve Jensen - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (4):415.
    The understanding of decision-making systems has come together in recent years to form a unified theory of decision-making in the mammalian brain as arising from multiple, interacting systems (a planning system, a habit system, and a situation-recognition system). This unified decision-making system has multiple potential access points through which it can be driven to make maladaptive choices, particularly choices that entail seeking of certain drugs or behaviors. We identify 10 key vulnerabilities in the system: (1) moving away from homeostasis, (2) (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Semicovert behavior and the concept of pain.Ullin T. Place - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):70-71.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ghostbusting.Howard Rachlin - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):73-83.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Stepping Into a Map: Initial Heading Direction Influences Spatial Memory Flexibility.Stephanie A. Gagnon, Tad T. Brunyé, Aaron Gardony, Matthijs L. Noordzij, Caroline R. Mahoney & Holly A. Taylor - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (2):275-302.
    Learning a novel environment involves integrating first-person perceptual and motoric experiences with developing knowledge about the overall structure of the surroundings. The present experiments provide insights into the parallel development of these egocentric and allocentric memories by intentionally conflicting body- and world-centered frames of reference during learning, and measuring outcomes via online and offline measures. Results of two experiments demonstrate faster learning and increased memory flexibility following route perspective reading (Experiment 1) and virtual navigation (Experiment 2) when participants begin exploring (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Introduction to the Topic on Modeling Spatial Cognition.Glenn Gunzelmann - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (4):628-631.
    Our ability to process spatial information is fundamental for understanding and interacting with the environment, and it pervades other components of cognitive functioning from language to mathematics. Moreover, technological advances have produced new capabilities that have created research opportunities and astonishing applications. In this Topic on Modeling Spatial Cognition, research crossing a variety of disciplines and methodologies is described, all focused on developing models to represent the capacities and limitations of human spatial cognition.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Animal Behavior.Stephen J. Crowley & Colin Allen - 2008 - In Michael Ruse (ed.), The Oxford handbook of philosophy of biology. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 327--348.
    Few areas of scientific investigation have spawned more alternative approaches than animal behavior: comparative psychology, ethology, behavioral ecology, sociobiology, behavioral endocrinology, behavioral neuroscience, neuroethology, behavioral genetics, cognitive ethology, developmental psychobiology---the list goes on. Add in the behavioral sciences focused on the human animal, and you can continue the list with ethnography, biological anthropology, political science, sociology, psychology (cognitive, social, developmental, evolutionary, etc.), and even that dismal science, economics. Clearly, no reasonable-length chapter can do justice to such a varied collection. We (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Cultural evolution.Robert Artigiani - 1987 - World Futures 23 (1):93-121.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Beyond Core Knowledge: Natural Geometry.Elizabeth Spelke, Sang Ah Lee & Véronique Izard - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (5):863-884.
    For many centuries, philosophers and scientists have pondered the origins and nature of human intuitions about the properties of points, lines, and figures on the Euclidean plane, with most hypothesizing that a system of Euclidean concepts either is innate or is assembled by general learning processes. Recent research from cognitive and developmental psychology, cognitive anthropology, animal cognition, and cognitive neuroscience suggests a different view. Knowledge of geometry may be founded on at least two distinct, evolutionarily ancient, core cognitive systems for (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • The mechanisms of human action: introduction and background.Ezequiel Morsella - 2009 - In Ezequiel Morsella, John A. Bargh & Peter M. Gollwitzer (eds.), Oxford handbook of human action. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 1--32.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Undermining the foundations: Questioning the basic notions of associationism and mental representation.Ezequiel Morsella, Travis A. Riddle & John A. Bargh - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (2):218-219.
    Perhaps the time has come to re-examine the basic notions of cognitive science. Together with previous challenges against associationism, the target article should be viewed as a call to arms to re-evaluate the empirical basis for contemporary conceptualizations of human learning and the notion of a concept that has become too imprecise for describing the elements of cognition.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The origins and governance of complex social systems.Robert Artigiani - 2004 - World Futures 60 (8):593 – 616.
    The new science of Complexity explains that limited knowledge prevents societies from predicting and controlling their developments. But Complexity further suggests that nature uses the limits of knowledge to evolve, which turns an apparent obstacle into an opportunity to reevaluate governmental institutions. As in nature, the limits of knowledge lead social systems to evolve by individuating, liberating, and empowering their members. Societies individuate and liberate their members to probe environments and exploit opportunities. Societies empower individuals to globalize their findings which (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The ephemeral stories of our lives.Nick Chater - 2023 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46:e89.
    Johnson et al. make a persuasive case that qualitative, story-like reasoning plays a crucial role in everyday thought and decision-making. This commentary questions the cohesiveness of this type of reasoning and the representations that generate it. Perhaps narratives do not underpin, but are ephemeral products of thought, created when we need to justify our actions to ourselves and others.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Using position rather than color at the traffic light – Covariation learning-based deviation from instructions in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder.Robert Gaschler, Beate Elisabeth Ditsche-Klein, Michael Kriechbaumer, Christine Blech & Dorit Wenke - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Based on instructions people can form task representations that shield relevant from seemingly irrelevant information. It has been documented that instructions can tie people to a particular way of performing a task despite that in principle a more efficient way could be learned and used. Since task shielding can lead to persistence of inefficient variants of task performance, it is relevant to test whether individuals with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder – characterized by less task shielding – are more likely and quicker (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modelling ourselves: what the free energy principle reveals about our implicit notions of representation.Matt Sims & Giovanni Pezzulo - 2021 - Synthese 199 (3-4):7801-7833.
    Predictive processing theories are increasingly popular in philosophy of mind; such process theories often gain support from the Free Energy Principle —a normative principle for adaptive self-organized systems. Yet there is a current and much discussed debate about conflicting philosophical interpretations of FEP, e.g., representational versus non-representational. Here we argue that these different interpretations depend on implicit assumptions about what qualifies as representational. We deploy the Free Energy Principle instrumentally to distinguish four main notions of representation, which focus on organizational, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • From animals to animats: Proceedings of the First International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior.Matthew Brand, Peter Prokopowicz & Clark Elliott - 1995 - Artificial Intelligence 73 (1-2):307-322.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Low-Resolution Place and Response Learning Capacities in Down Syndrome.Mathilde Bostelmann, Floriana Costanzo, Lorelay Martorana, Deny Menghini, Stefano Vicari, Pamela Banta Lavenex & Pierre Lavenex - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Down syndrome (DS), the most common genetic cause of intellectual disability, results from the partial or complete triplication of chromosome 21. Individuals with DS are impaired at using a high-resolution, allocentric spatial representation to learn and remember discrete locations in a controlled environment. Here, we assessed the capacity of individuals with DS to perform low-resolution spatial learning, depending on two competing memory systems: (1) the place learning system, which depends on the hippocampus and creates flexible relational representations of the environment; (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The hippocampus and spatial constraints on mental imagery.Chris M. Bird, James A. Bisby & Neil Burgess - 2012 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Thought without Language: Thought without Awareness?L. Weiskrantz - 1997 - Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement 42:127-150.
    Some philosophers have laid down rather severe strictures on whether there can be thought without language. Wittgenstein asserted that ‘the limits of language…mean the limits of my world’. Davidson has argued that ‘a creature cannot have thoughts unless it is an interpreter of the speech of another’. Dummett has interpreted some pronouncements as meaning that ‘the study of thought is to be sharply distinguished from the study of the psychological processes of thinking and…the only proper method of analysing thought consists (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The analysis of verbal behavior.John B. Carroll - 1944 - Psychological Review 51 (2):102-119.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark