Switch to: References

Citations of:

Features of similarity

Psychological Review 84 (4):327-352 (1977)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Contextual effects on encoding and recognition of category members subsumed by two levels of classification.Lorraine A. Low - 1988 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 26 (4):301-304.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • When learning to classify by relations is easier than by features.Bradley C. Love & Marc T. Tomlinson - 2010 - Thinking and Reasoning 16 (4):372-401.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The Measure of Perceived Similarity Between Faces: Old Issues for a New Method.Ludovica Lorusso, Luca Pulina & Enrico Grosso - 2015 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 6 (2):317-339.
    Measuring perceived similarity is an important issue in visual perception of faces, since a measure of the perceived similarity between faces may be used to investigate fundamental tasks like face categorization and recognition. Despite its fundamental role, measuring perceived similarity between faces is not trivial from both a theoretical and methodological point of view. In this paper we present theoretical arguments that undermine the method currently most used to measure perceived similarity between faces in visual perception, and we propose an (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Paradox of Graded Justification.Artūrs Logins - forthcoming - Episteme:1-29.
    According to a widely held view epistemic justification is a normative notion. According to another widely held assumption, epistemic justification comes in degrees. Given that gradability requires a context-sensitivity that normativity seems to lack, these two assumptions stand in tension. Giving up the assumption of gradability of justification represents a lesser theoretical cost.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • An instance theory of attention and memory.Gordon D. Logan - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (2):376-400.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • Recoding processes in memory.Elizabeth F. Loftus & Jonathan W. Schooler - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):246.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Structure Mapping and Vocabularies for Thinking.Jeffrey Loewenstein - 2017 - Topics in Cognitive Science 9 (3):842-858.
    While extremes tend to capture attention, the ordinary is often most of the story. So it may be with the structure-mapping process. The structure-mapping process can account for such pinnacles of thinking as analogy and metaphor, which can lead to overlooking the mundane, incremental use of structure mapping. Consequently, the current discussion shifts focus to the value of close comparisons between literally similar items for the development of knowledge. The intent is to foster greater integration between process and content as (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Concepts, categories, and epistemology.Kenneth R. Livingston - 1989 - Philosophia 19 (2-3):265-300.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The effect of preference learning on context effects in multi-alternative, multi-attribute choice.Yanjun Liu & Jennifer S. Trueblood - 2023 - Cognition 233 (C):105365.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sequential information processing in persuasion.Roman Linne, Jannis Hildebrandt, Gerd Bohner & Hans-Peter Erb - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    We present a theory of sequential information processing in persuasion. It extends assumptions of the heuristic-systematic model, in particular the idea that information encountered early in a persuasion situation may affect the processing of subsequent information. SIP also builds on the abstraction from content-related dichotomies in accord with the parametric unimodel of social judgment. SIP features one constitutional axiom and three main postulates: Persuasion is the sequential processing of information that is relevant to judgment formation. Inferences drawn from initial information (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On learnability, empirical foundations, and naturalness.W. J. M. Levelt - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):501-501.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Why it is unsurprising that ape “language training” enhances “completing incomplete (external) representations of action”.Justin Leiber - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):151-151.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Metaphors, Stories, Models: A Unified Account of Decisions.Johannes M. Lehner - 2002 - Philosophy of Management 2 (1):35-46.
    Making decisions, as Peter Drucker put it, ‘is the specific executive task’.2 But the situations in which managers decide can differ sharply. Some involve risk, uncertainty or lack of predictability while others lack clear structure and present decision-makers with ambiguity in some form. And yet, in spite of much research, we still have no unified account to explain how managers make decisions let alone to help them decide effectively. Different research streams specialise in different aspects of judgement and decisionmaking (JDM) (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Metaphors, Stories, Models: A Unified Account of Decisions.Johannes M. Lehner - 2002 - Philosophy of Management 2 (1):35-46.
    Making decisions, as Peter Drucker put it, ‘is the specific executive task’.2 But the situations in which managers decide can differ sharply. Some involve risk, uncertainty or lack of predictability while others lack clear structure and present decision-makers with ambiguity in some form. And yet, in spite of much research, we still have no unified account to explain how managers make decisions let alone to help them decide effectively. Different research streams specialise in different aspects of judgement and decisionmaking (JDM) (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Metaphors, Stories, Models: A Unified Account of Decisions.Johannes M. Lehner - 2002 - Philosophy of Management 2 (2):11-20.
    Part 1 of this paper1 used the notions of equivocality and uncertainty to distinguish the situations in which managers make judgements and decisions and described in general how managers use models in these different contexts. This final second part describes in detail the three types of models managers use: formal models, stories and metaphors. It offers five propositions about how managers use the three types of model, propositions which can usefully form the basis of future empirical research.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • When explanation is too hard (or understanding hijacking for novices).Michael Lebowitz - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):662-663.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Categorizing Numeric Information for Generalization.Michael Lebowitz - 1985 - Cognitive Science 9 (3):285-308.
    Learning programs that generalize from real‐world examples will have to deal with many different kinds of data. Continuous numeric data can cause problems for algorithms that search for examples with identical property values. These problems can be surmounted by categorizing the numeric data. However, this process has problems of its own. In this paper, we look at the need for categorizing numeric data and several methods for doing so. We concentrate on the use of generalization‐based memory, a memory organization where (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Processes of Similarity Judgment.Levi B. Larkey & Arthur B. Markman - 2005 - Cognitive Science 29 (6):1061-1076.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • CAB: Connectionist Analogy Builder.Levi B. Larkey & Bradley C. Love - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (5):781-794.
    The ability to make informative comparisons is central to human cognition. Comparison involves aligning two representations and placing their elements into correspondence. Detecting correspondences is a necessary component of analogical inference, recognition, categorization, schema formation, and similarity judgment. Connectionist Analogy Builder (CAB) determines correspondences through a simple iterative computation that matches elements in one representation with elements playing compatible roles in the other representation while simultaneously enforcing structural constraints. CAB shows promise as a process model of comparison as its performance (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • The encoding of spatial position in the brain.Joseph S. Lappin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):74-75.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • New failures to learn.Barbara Landau - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):660-661.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Induction and explanation: Complementary models of learning.Pat Langley - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):661-662.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Approaches to learning and representation.Pat Langley - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):500-501.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge.Thomas K. Landauer & Susan T. Dumais - 1997 - Psychological Review 104 (2):211-240.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   340 citations  
  • What can psychologists learn from hidden-unit nets?K. Lamberts & G. D'Ydewalle - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):499-500.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Epistemic Gradualism Versus Epistemic Absolutism.Changsheng Lai - 2021 - Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 103 (1):186-207.
    Epistemic absolutism holds that knowledge‐that is ungradable, while epistemic gradualism argues the opposite. This paper purports to remodel the gradualism/absolutism debate. The current model initiated by Stephen Hetherington fails to capture the genuine divergence between the two views, which makes the debate equivocal, and the gradualist side lacks appeal. I propose that the remodeled debate should focus on whether knowledge‐that is a ‘threshold concept’ or a ‘spectrum concept’. That is, whether there is a threshold distinguishing knowledge from non‐knowledge. The reconstructed (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The episodic/semantic continuum in an evolved machine.Roy Lachman & Mary J. Naus - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):244.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How Many Mechanisms Are Needed to Analyze Speech? A Connectionist Simulation of Structural Rule Learning in Artificial Language Acquisition.Aarre Laakso & Paco Calvo - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (7):1243-1281.
    Some empirical evidence in the artificial language acquisition literature has been taken to suggest that statistical learning mechanisms are insufficient for extracting structural information from an artificial language. According to the more than one mechanism (MOM) hypothesis, at least two mechanisms are required in order to acquire language from speech: (a) a statistical mechanism for speech segmentation; and (b) an additional rule-following mechanism in order to induce grammatical regularities. In this article, we present a set of neural network studies demonstrating (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Induction and probability.Henry E. Kyburg - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):660-660.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Detecting anomalous features in complex stimuli: The role of structured comparison.Kenneth J. Kurtz & Dedre Gentner - 2013 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 19 (3):219.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • What does Ghiselin mean by “individual”?Joseph B. Kruskal - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):294-295.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • How connectionist models learn: The course of learning in connectionist networks.John K. Kruschke - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):498-499.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Varieties of sameness: the impact of relational complexity on perceptual comparisons.James K. Kroger, Keith J. Holyoak & John E. Hummel - 2004 - Cognitive Science 28 (3):335-358.
    The fundamental relations that underlie cognitive comparisons—“same” and “different”—can be defined at multiple levels of abstraction, which vary in relational complexity. We compared response times to decide whether or not two sequentially‐presented patterns, each composed of two pairs of colored squares, were the same at three levels of abstraction: perceptual, relational, and system (higher order relations). For both 150 ms and 5 s inter‐stimulus intervals (ISIs), both with and without a masking stimulus, decision time increased with level of abstraction. Sameness (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Varieties of sameness: the impact of relational complexity on perceptual comparisons*1.J. Kroger - 2004 - Cognitive Science 28 (3):335-358.
    The fundamental relations that underlie cognitive comparisons—“same” and “different”—can be defined at multiple levels of abstraction, which vary in relational complexity. We compared response times to decide whether or not two sequentially‐presented patterns, each composed of two pairs of colored squares, were the same at three levels of abstraction: perceptual, relational, and system (higher order relations). For both 150 ms and 5 s inter‐stimulus intervals (ISIs), both with and without a masking stimulus, decision time increased with level of abstraction. Sameness (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Representational geometry: integrating cognition, computation, and the brain.Nikolaus Kriegeskorte & Rogier A. Kievit - 2013 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 (8):401-412.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • A provisional sensory/motor “complementarity” model for adaptation effects.Ivo Kohler - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):73-74.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Armchair theorists have more fun.Roberta L. Klatzky - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):244.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The personal is philosophical is political: A philosopher and mother of a cognitively disabled person sends notes from the battlefield.Eva Feder Kittay - 2009 - Metaphilosophy 40 (3-4):606-627.
    Having encountered landmines in offering a critique of philosophy based on my experience as the mother of a cognitively disabled daughter, I ask, “Should I continue?” I defend the idea that pursuing this project is of a piece with the invisible care labor that is done by people with disabilities and their families. The value of attempting to influence philosophical conceptions of cognitive disability by virtue of this experience is justified by an inextricable relationship between the personal, the political, and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Second-generation AI theories of learning.David Kirsh - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):658-659.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The exemplars of a strong whole were rated as more similar than were the exemplars of a weak whole.Donald L. King - 1987 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 25 (1):51-53.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Young Children's Reliance on Information From Inaccurate Informants.Sunae Kim, Markus Paulus & Chuck Kalish - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (S3):601-621.
    Prior work shows that children selectively learn from credible speakers. Yet little is known how they treat information from non-credible speakers. This research examined to what extent and under what conditions children may or may not learn from problematic sources. In three studies, we found that children displayed trust toward previously inaccurate speakers. Children were equally likely to extend labels from previously accurate and inaccurate speakers to novel objects. Moreover, they expected third parties to share labels provided by previously inaccurate (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Social Comparison and Distributive Justice: East Asia Differences.Tae-Yeol Kim, Jeffrey R. Edwards & Debra L. Shapiro - 2015 - Journal of Business Ethics 132 (2):401-414.
    Using a survey of 393 employees who were natives and residents of China, Japan, and South Korea, we examined the extent to which employees from different countries within East Asia experience distributive justice when they perceived that their work outcomes relative to a referent other were equally poor, equally favorable, more poor, or more favorable. As predicted, we found that when employees perceived themselves relative to a referent other to be recipients of more favorable outcomes, Chinese and Korean employees were (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Dynamics Versus Development in Numerosity Estimation: A Computational Model Accurately Predicts a Developmental Reversal.Dan Kim & John E. Opfer - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (10):e13049.
    Perceptual judgments result from a dynamic process, but little is known about the dynamics of number‐line estimation. A recent study proposed a computational model that combined a model of trial‐to‐trial changes with a model for the internal scaling of discrete numbers. Here, we tested a surprising prediction of the model—a situation in which children's estimates of numerosity would be better than those of adults. Consistent with the model simulations, task contexts led to a clear developmental reversal: children made more adult‐like, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A fact is a fact is a fact.John F. Kihlstrom - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):243.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Getting Serious about Shared Features.Donal Khosrowi - 2020 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 71 (2):523-546.
    In Simulation and Similarity, Michael Weisberg offers a similarity-based account of the model–world relation, which is the relation in virtue of which successful models are successful. Weisberg’s main idea is that models are similar to targets in virtue of sharing features. An important concern about Weisberg’s account is that it remains silent on what it means for models and targets to share features, and consequently on how feature-sharing contributes to models’ epistemic success. I consider three potential ways of concretizing the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Exploring the conceptual universe.Charles Kemp - 2012 - Psychological Review 119 (4):685-722.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Motor-sensory feedback formulations: are we asking the right questions?J. A. Scott Kelso - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):72-73.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Clarity, generality, and efficiency in models of learning: Wringing the MOP.Kevin T. Kelly - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):657-658.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Natural categories and natural concepts.Frank C. Keil - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):293-294.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A probabilistic framework for memory-based reasoning.Simon Kasif, Steven Salzberg, David Waltz, John Rachlin & David W. Aha - 1998 - Artificial Intelligence 104 (1-2):287-311.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark