Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. A reassessment of the shift from the classical theory of concepts to prototype theory.Eric Margolis - 1994 - Cognition 51 (1):73-89.
    A standard view within psychology is that there have been two important shifts in the study of concepts and that each has led to some improvements. The first shift was from the classical theory of concepts to probabilistic theories, including the prototype theory. The second shift was from probabilistic theories to theory-based theories. In this article, I critically evaluate the view that the first shift was a major advance and argue that the prototype theory suffers some of the same problems (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • The structure of a semantic theory.Jerrold Katz & Jerry Fodor - 1963 - Language 39:170-210.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   157 citations  
  • Conceptual Differences Between Children and Adults.Susan Carey - 1988 - Mind and Language 3 (3):167-181.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   142 citations  
  • Explaining Language Use.Noam Chomsky - 1992 - Philosophical Topics 20 (1):205-231.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • (1 other version)Meaning and reference.Hilary Putnam - 1973 - Journal of Philosophy 70 (19):699-711.
    UNCLEAR as it is, the traditional doctrine that the notion "meaning" possesses the extension/intension ambiguity has certain typical consequences. The doctrine that the meaning of a term is a concept carried the implication that mean- ings are mental entities. Frege, however, rebelled against this "psy- chologism." Feeling that meanings are public property-that the same meaning can be "grasped" by more than one person and by persons at different times-he identified concepts (and hence "intensions" or meanings) with abstract entities rather than (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   384 citations  
  • (1 other version)Defining art historically.Jerrold Levinson - 1979 - British Journal of Aesthetics 19 (3):21-33.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   86 citations  
  • (1 other version)The effect of essentialism on taxonomy—two thousand years of stasis.David L. Hull - 1964 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 15 (60):314-326.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   160 citations  
  • The present status of the innateness controversy.Jerry A. Fodor - 1981 - In Representations: philosophical essays on the foundations of cognitive science. Cambridge: MIT Press. pp. 257-316.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   173 citations  
  • (1 other version)The meaning of 'meaning'.Hilary Putnam - 1975 - Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science 7:131-193.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1431 citations  
  • Pattern and being.John Haugeland - 1993 - In B. Dahlbom (ed.), Dennett and His Critics: Demystifying Mind. Cambridge: Blackwell.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • (1 other version)Natural Kinds and Biological Taxa.John Dupré - 1981 - The Philosophical Review 90 (1):66-90.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   178 citations  
  • Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding.Irving Biederman - 1987 - Psychological Review 94 (2):115-147.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   539 citations  
  • Constraints Children Place on Word Meanings.Ellen M. Markman - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (1):57-77.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   89 citations  
  • What some concepts might not be.Sharon Lee Armstrong, Lila R. Gleitman & Henry Gleitman - 1983 - Cognition 13 (1):263--308.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   242 citations  
  • (1 other version)The role of theories in conceptual coherence.G. L. Murphy & D. L. Medin - 1999 - In Eric Margolis & Stephen Laurence (eds.), Concepts: Core Readings. MIT Press. pp. 289--316.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   278 citations  
  • Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories.Eleanor Rosch & Carolyn Mervis - 1975 - Cognitive Psychology 7 (4):573--605.
    Six experiments explored the hypothesis that the members of categories which are considered most prototypical are those with most attributes in common with other members of the category and least attributes in common with other categories. In probabilistic terms, the hypothesis is that prototypicality is a function of the total cue validity of the attributes of items. In Experiments 1 and 3, subjects listed attributes for members of semantic categories which had been previously rated for degree of prototypicality. High positive (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   402 citations  
  • Perception, ontology, and word meaning.Nancy N. Soja, Susan Carey & Elizabeth S. Spelke - 1992 - Cognition 45 (1):101-107.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Syntactic cues in the acquisition of collective nouns.Paul Bloom & Deborah Kelemen - 1995 - Cognition 56 (1):1-30.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Water is not H 2 O.B. C. Malt - 1994 - Cognitive Psychology 27:41--70.
    What makes a liquid water? A strong version of ``psychological essentialis'' predicts that people use the presence or absence of H2O as the primary determinant of what liquids they call ``water.'' To test this prediction, subjects were asked to judge the amount of H2O in liquids called ``water'' and liquids not called ``water.'' Neither their beliefs about the simple presence/absence of H2O nor about the proportion of H2O in the liquids accounted well for which ones are normally called "water." Typicality (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   56 citations  
  • (1 other version)Refining art historically.Jerrold Levinson - 1989 - Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 47 (1):21-33.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • (1 other version)The role of theories in conceptual coherence.Gregory L. Murphy & Douglas L. Medin - 1985 - Psychological Review 92 (3):289-316.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   489 citations  
  • (1 other version)Natural kinds and biological taxa.John Dupré - 1981 - Philosophical Review 90 (1):66-90.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   209 citations  
  • Nondescriptionality and natural kind terms.Barbara Abbott - 1989 - Linguistics and Philosophy 12 (3):269 - 291.
    The phrase "natural kind term" has come into the linguistic and philosophical literature in connection with well-known work of Kripke (1972) and Putnam (1970, 1975a). I use that phrase here in the sense it has acquired from those and subseqnent works on related topics. This is not the transparent sense of the phrase. That is, if I am right in what follows there are words for kinds of things existing in nature which are not natural kind terms in the current (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Titles.Jerrold Levison - 1985 - Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 44 (1):29-39.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Initial knowledge: six suggestions.Elizabeth Spelke - 1994 - Cognition 50 (1-3):431-445.
    Although debates continue, studies of cognition in infancy suggest that knowledge begins to emerge early in life and constitutes part of humans' innate endowment. Early-developing knowledge appears to be both domain-specific and task-specific, it appears to capture fundamental constraints on ecologically important classes of entities in the child's environment, and it appears to remain central to the commonsense knowledge systems of adults.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   147 citations  
  • Categories and induction in young children.Susan A. Gelman & Ellen M. Markman - 1986 - Cognition 23 (3):183-209.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   220 citations  
  • Extending art historically.Jerrold Levinson - 1993 - Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 51 (3):411-423.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • Introduction.Steven Schwartz - 1977 - In Stephen P. Schwartz (ed.), Naming, necessity, and natural kinds. Ithaca [N.Y.]: Cornell University Press. pp. 13-41.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • The Nature and Development of the Teleological Stance.Deborah Ann Kelemen - 1996 - Dissertation, The University of Arizona
    Teleological reasoning--reasoning that is based on the assumption of purpose, design or function--is a fundamental aspect of adult cognition. It leads us to think about the actions of others in terms of goals and to presume that people's creations are designed for a purpose. It also causes us to reason about biological entities in terms of functions and characterizes much of our metaphysical musing on life and meaning. This dissertation explores the nature and development of teleological thought in preschool children. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations