Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Toward a cognitive psychology of syntax: Information processing contributions to sentence formulation.J. Kathryn Bock - 1982 - Psychological Review 89 (1):1-47.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   86 citations  
  • A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production.Gary S. Dell - 1986 - Psychological Review 93 (3):283-321.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   248 citations  
  • Lexical access and inflectional morphology.Alfonso Caramazza, Alessandro Laudanna & Cristina Romani - 1988 - Cognition 28 (3):297-332.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Wordshape errors in language production.Joseph Paul Stemberger - 1990 - Cognition 35 (2):123-157.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • (1 other version)Grammar‐based Connectionist Approaches to Language.Paul Smolensky - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):589-613.
    This article describes an approach to connectionist language research that relies on the development of grammar formalisms rather than computer models. From formulations of the fundamental theoretical commitments of connectionism and of generative grammar, it is argued that these two paradigms are mutually compatible. Integrating the basic assumptions of the paradigms results in formal theories of grammar that centrally incorporate a certain degree of connectionist computation. Two such grammar formalisms—Harmonic Grammar and Optimality Theory —are briefly introduced to illustrate grammar‐based approaches (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection.Harald Clahsen - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):991-1013.
    Following much work in linguistic theory, it is hypothesized that the language faculty has a modular structure and consists of two basic components, a lexicon of (structured) entries and a computational system of combinatorial operations to form larger linguistic expressions from lexical entries. This target article provides evidence for the dual nature of the language faculty by describing recent results of a multidisciplinary investigation of German inflection. We have examined: (1) its linguistic representation, focussing on noun plurals and verb inflection (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perception: Implications for child language acquisition.Kim Plunkett & Virginia Marchman - 1991 - Cognition 38 (1):43-102.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   181 citations  
  • A Connectionist Model of English Past Tense and Plural Morphology.Kim Plunkett & Patrick Juola - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):463-490.
    The acquisition of English noun and verb morphology is modeled using a single-system connectionist network. The network is trained to produce the plurals and past tense forms of a large corpus of monosyllabic English nouns and verbs. The developmental trajectory of network performance is analyzed in detail and is shown to mimic a number of important features of the acquisition of English noun and verb morphology in young children. These include an initial error-free period of performance on both nouns and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition.Steven Pinker & Alan Prince - 1988 - Cognition 28 (1-2):73-193.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   378 citations  
  • (1 other version)Grammar‐based Connectionist Approaches to Language.P. K. Monteiro, M. R. Pascoa & P. Smolensky - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):589-613.
    This article describes an approach to connectionist language research that relies on the development of grammar formalisms rather than computer models. From formulations of the fundamental theoretical commitments of connectionism and of generative grammar, it is argued that these two paradigms are mutually compatible. Integrating the basic assumptions of the paradigms results in formal theories of grammar that centrally incorporate a certain degree of connectionist computation. Two such grammar formalisms—Harmonic Grammar and Optimality Theory —are briefly introduced to illustrate grammar‐based approaches (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Rules, representations, and the English past tense.William Marslen-Wilson & Lorraine K. Tyler - 1998 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2 (11):428-435.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • (1 other version)Children's Productivity in the English Past Tense: The Role of Frequency, Phonology, and Neighborhood Structure.Virginia A. Marchman - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (3):283-304.
    The productive use of English past tense morphology in school‐aged children (N = 74; 3 years, 8 months to 13 years, 5 months) is explored using an elicited production task. Errors represented 20% of the responses overall. Virtually all of the children demonstrated productivity with regular (e.g., good) and irregular patterns (zero‐marking, e.g., sit → sit; vowel‐change, e.g., ride → rid). Overall frequency of errors decreased with age, yet the tendency for certain types of irregularizations increased in the older groups. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • (1 other version)Children's Frequency , Productivity Phonology, in the and English Past Tense : The Role of Neighborhood Structure.Virginia A. Marchman - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (3):283-304.
    The productive use of English past tense morphology in school-aged children (N= 74; 3 years, 8 months to 13 years, 5 months) is explored using on elicited production task. Errors represented 20% of the responses overall. Virtually all of the children demonstrated productivity with regular (e.g., good) and irregular patterns (zero-marking, e.g., sit + sit; vowel-change, e.g., ride -+ rid). Overall frequency of errors decreased with age, yet the tendency for certain types of irregularizations increased in the older groups. Analyses (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Implementations are not conceptualizations: Revising the verb learning model.Brian MacWhinney & Jared Leinbach - 1991 - Cognition 40 (1-2):121-157.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   99 citations  
  • (1 other version)Why No Mere Mortal Has Ever Flown Out to Center Field.John J. Kim, Steven Pinker, Alan Prince & Sandeep Prasada - 1991 - Cognitive Science 15 (2):173-218.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  • (1 other version)Why No Mere Mortal Has Ever Flown Out to Center Field.John J. Kim, Steven Pinker, Alan Prince & Sandeep Prasada - 1992 - Cognitive Science 16 (1):151-151.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Level-ordering in lexical development.Peter Gordon - 1985 - Cognition 21 (2):73-93.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   74 citations  
  • Structure and Content in Language Production: A Theory of Frame Constraints in Phonological Speech Errors.Gary S. Dell, Cornell Juliano & Anita Govindjee - 1993 - Cognitive Science 17 (2):149-195.
    Theories of language production propose that utterances are constructed by a mechanism that separates linguistic content from linguistic structure, Linguistic content is retrieved from the mental lexicon, and is then inserted into slots in linguistic structures or frames. Support for this kind of model at the phonological level comes from patterns of phonological speech errors. W present an alternative account of these patterns using a connectionist or parallel distributed proceesing (PDP) model that learns to produce sequences of phonological features. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations