Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Integrating experiential and distributional data to learn semantic representations.Mark Andrews, Gabriella Vigliocco & David Vinson - 2009 - Psychological Review 116 (3):463-498.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings.James L. McClelland & David E. Rumelhart - 1981 - Psychological Review 88 (5):375-407.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   637 citations  
  • A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge.Thomas K. Landauer & Susan T. Dumais - 1997 - Psychological Review 104 (2):211-240.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   350 citations  
  • Conceptual Hierarchies in a Flat Attractor Network: Dynamics of Learning and Computations.Christopher M. O’Connor, George S. Cree & Ken McRae - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (4):665-708.
    The structure of people’s conceptual knowledge of concrete nouns has traditionally been viewed as hierarchical (Collins & Quillian, 1969). For example, superordinate concepts (vegetable) are assumed to reside at a higher level than basic‐level concepts (carrot). A feature‐based attractor network with a single layer of semantic features developed representations of both basic‐level and superordinate concepts. No hierarchical structure was built into the network. In Experiment and Simulation 1, the graded structure of categories (typicality ratings) is accounted for by the flat (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The Big Book of Concepts.Gregory Murphy - 2004 - MIT Press.
    A comprehensive introduction to current research on the psychology of concept formation and use.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   297 citations  
  • The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning.Bernhard Hommel, Jochen Müsseler, Gisa Aschersleben & Wolfgang Prinz - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (5):849-878.
    Traditional approaches to human information processing tend to deal with perception and action planning in isolation, so that an adequate account of the perception-action interface is still missing. On the perceptual side, the dominant cognitive view largely underestimates, and thus fails to account for, the impact of action-related processes on both the processing of perceptual information and on perceptual learning. On the action side, most approaches conceive of action planning as a mere continuation of stimulus processing, thus failing to account (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   347 citations  
  • (1 other version)Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recognition and recall.Antonio R. Damasio - 1989 - Cognition 3 (1-2):25-62.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   222 citations  
  • Using movement and intentions to understand simple events.Jeffrey M. Zacks - 2004 - Cognitive Science 28 (6):979-1008.
    In order to understand ongoing activity, observers segment it into meaningful temporal parts. Segmentation can be based on bottom‐up processing of distinctive sensory characteristics, such as movement features. Segmentation may also be affected by top‐down effects of knowledge structures, including information about actors' intentions. Three experiments investigated the role of movement features and intentions in perceptual event segmentation, using simple animations. In all conditions, movement features significantly predicted where participants segmented. This relationship was stronger when participants identified larger units than (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   44 citations  
  • The Situated Nature of Concepts.Lawerence Barsalou & Wenchi Yeh - 2006 - American Journal of Psychology 119:349-384.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • (1 other version)Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition.Antonio R. Damasio - 1989 - Cognition 33 (1-2):25-62.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   109 citations  
  • The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model.Walter Kintsch - 1988 - Psychological Review 95 (2):163-182.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   200 citations  
  • Gist of the scene.Aude Oliva - 2005 - In Laurent Itti, Geraint Rees & John K. Tsotsos (eds.), Neurobiology of Attention. Academic Press. pp. 696--64.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations.David E. Meyer & Roger W. Schvaneveldt - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 90 (2):227.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   280 citations  
  • On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning.Ken McRae, Virginia R. de Sa & Mark S. Seidenberg - 1997 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 126 (2):99-130.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   66 citations  
  • Structure and process in semantic memory: A featural model for semantic decisions.Edward E. Smith, Edward J. Shoben & Lance J. Rips - 1974 - Psychological Review 81 (3):214-241.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   160 citations  
  • A Computational Model of Event Segmentation From Perceptual Prediction.Jeremy R. Reynolds, Jeffrey M. Zacks & Todd S. Braver - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (4):613-643.
    People tend to perceive ongoing continuous activity as series of discrete events. This partitioning of continuous activity may occur, in part, because events correspond to dynamic patterns that have recurred across different contexts. Recurring patterns may lead to reliable sequential dependencies in observers' experiences, which then can be used to guide perception. The current set of simulations investigated whether this statistical structure within events can be used 1) to develop stable internal representations that facilitate perception and 2) to learn when (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Connectionist modelling in psychology: A localist manifesto.Mike Page - 2000 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (4):443-467.
    Over the last decade, fully distributed models have become dominant in connectionist psychological modelling, whereas the virtues of localist models have been underestimated. This target article illustrates some of the benefits of localist modelling. Localist models are characterized by the presence of localist representations rather than the absence of distributed representations. A generalized localist model is proposed that exhibits many of the properties of fully distributed models. It can be applied to a number of problems that are difficult for fully (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • Recall and recognition of pictures by children as a function of organization and distractor similarity.Jean M. Mandler & Nancy L. Stein - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 102 (4):657.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts.L. W. Barsalou - 1982 - Memory and Cognition 10:82-93.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   74 citations  
  • Language Encodes Geographical Information.Max M. Louwerse & Rolf A. Zwaan - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (1):51-73.
    Population counts and longitude and latitude coordinates were estimated for the 50 largest cities in the United States by computational linguistic techniques and by human participants. The mathematical technique Latent Semantic Analysis applied to newspaper texts produced similarity ratings between the 50 cities that allowed for a multidimensional scaling (MDS) of these cities. MDS coordinates correlated with the actual longitude and latitude of these cities, showing that cities that are located together share similar semantic contexts. This finding was replicated using (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations